Clarifying the Role of Mentalization in the Development of Executive Functions
HTML Full Text (Spanish)
PDF (Spanish)
XML (Spanish)

Keywords

executive functions
mentalization
prefrontal cortex
behaviour
adaptation

How to Cite

Clarifying the Role of Mentalization in the Development of Executive Functions. (2017). Universitas Psychologica, 16(4), 1-19. https://doi.org/10.11144/Javeriana.upsy16-4.crmd
Almetrics
 
Dimensions
 

Google Scholar
 
Search GoogleScholar

Abstract

Executive functions are a set of abilities that allow the production of adaptive behaviours to specific challenges from the environment. Mentalization refers to the way in which humans are able to discern mental states, intentions and beliefs of others through observed behaviour. This paper clarifies the way in which we should understand the relationship between these two concepts. After presenting some of the most fundamental aspects of these concepts, it is proposed that mentalization underlies, organizes, and enhances the development of executive functions, this, contrary to the dominant approach defended by Carlson Moses, and Brenton (2002) that suggests that executive functions underlie the development of mentalization. After reviewing empirical evidence for the defended alternative, the last section explores some of its applications in areas such as the study of the neurophysiological nature of the social, the subjective nature of consciousness, and the domain of psychopathology.

HTML Full Text (Spanish)
PDF (Spanish)
XML (Spanish)

Andreasen, N., & Pierson, R. (2008). The role of the cerebellum in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 64(2), 81-83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.01.003

Ardila, A., & Ostrosky-Solís, F. (2008). Desarrollo histórico de las funciones ejecutivas. Neuropsicología, Neuropsiquiatría y Neurociencia, 8(1), 1-21.

Báez, S., Marengo, J., Pérez, A., Huepe, D., Font, F.G., Rial, V., … Ibáñez, A. (2015). Theory of mind and its relationship with executive functions and emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Neuropsychology, 9(2), 203-18. https://doi.org/10.1111/jnp.12046

Baillieux, H., De Smetha, H. J., Paquier, P. F., De Deyn, P. P., & Mariën, P. (2008). Cerebellar neurocognition: Insights into the bottom of the brain. Clinical Neurology and Neurosurgery, 110(8), 763-773. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clineuro.2008.05.013

Baker, L. R. (2000). Persons and bodies. A constitution view. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Baron-Cohen, S. (2014). Autismo y Síndrome de Asperger (2.a ed.). Madrid: Alianza.

Baron-Cohen, S., Lombardo, M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2013). Understanding other minds: Perspective from developmental social neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Baron-Cohen, S. (2012) Empatía Cero. Nueva teoría de la crueldad. Alianza. Madrid, España.

Batson, C. D., Early, S., & Salvarani, G. (1997). Perspective taking: Imagining how another feels versus imaging how you would feel. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23(7), 751-758. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167297237008

Bauselas, E. (2014). Funciones ejecutivas: nociones del desarrollo desde una perspectiva neuropsicológica. Acción Psicológica, 11(1), 21-34.

Bayne, T. (2011). The sense of agency. En F. Macpherson (Ed.), The senses (pp. 355-374). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Bull, R., Phillips, L. H., & Conway, C. A. (2008). The role of control functions in mentalizing: Dual-task studies of theory of mind and executive function. Cognition, 107(2), 663-672. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.07.015

Cahill, C., & Frith, C. D. (1996). A cognitive basis for the signs and symptoms of schizophrenia. En C. Pantelis, H. E. Nelson & T. Barnes (Eds.), Schizophrenia: A Neuropsychological Perspective (pp. 373-395). Nueva York: John Wiley.

Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Brenton, C. (2002). How specific is the relation between executive function and theory of mind? Contributions of inhibitory control and working memory. Infant and Child Development, 11, 73-92. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.298

Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(3), 181-204. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12000477

Cook, R., Bird, G., Catmur, C., Press, C., & Heyes, C. (2014). Mirror neurons: Fron origin to function. Behavioral and Brain Science, 37(2), 177-241. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X13000903

Corlett, P. R., Murray, G. K., Honey, G. D., Aitken, M. R., Shanks, D. R., Robbins, T. W., … , Fletcher, P. C. (2007). Disrupted prediction-error signal in psychosis: Evidence for an associative account of delusions. Brain. A Journal of Neurology, 130(9), 2387-2400. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awm173

Corlett, P. R., Taylor, J. R., Wang, X. -J., Fletcher, P. C., & Krystal, J. H. (2010). Toward a neurobiology of delusions. Progress in Neurobiology, 92, 345-369. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2010.06.007

Damasio, A. (2010). El error de Descartes. La emoción, la razón y el cerebro humano. Barcelona: Crítica.

Damasio, A., & Meyer, K. (2008). Behind the looking-glass. Nature, 454(10), 167-168. https://doi.org/10.1038/454167a.

De Vignemont, F., & Fourneret, P. (2004). The sense of agency: A philosophical and empirical review of the “Who” system. Consciousness and Cognition, 13, 1-19. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8100(03)00022-9

De Vignemont, F., & Singer, T. (2006). The empathic brain: How, when and why? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(10), 435-441. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.08.008

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive function. Annual Review Psychology, 64, 135-168. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

Domínguez-Carral, J., Carreras-Sáez, I., García-Peñas, J., Fournier-Del Castillo, C., & Villalobos-Reales, J. (2015). Síndrome afectivo-cognitivo cerebeloso secundario a Tumor. An Pediatr, 82(1). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anpedi.2014.05.002

Dorado, C. (2012). Funciones cognitivas del cerebelo: implicación en las funciones ejecutivas. Revista Chilena de Neuropsicología, 7(2), 48-53. https://doi.org/10.5839/rcnp.2012.0702.02

Ferrari, P. F., & Rizzolatti, G. (2014). Mirror neuron research: The past and the future. Philosophical Transactions of The Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1644). https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0169

Fine, C., Lumsden, J., & Blair, R. (2001). Dissociation between ‘theory of mind’ and executive functions in a patient with early left amygdala damage. Brain. A Journal of Neurology, 124, 287-298. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/124.2.287

Finlay, L. (2005). “Reflexive embodied empathy”: A phenomenology of participant-researcher intersubjectivity. The Humanistic Psychologist, 33(4), 271-292. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15473333thp3304_4

Friston, K. J. (2010). The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(2), 127-138. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2787

Friedman, N., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., Corley, R. P., & Hewitt, J. K. (2008). Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. Journal of experimental Psychology General, 137(2), 201-225. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.201.

Frith, C. (1992). The cognitive neuropsychology of schizophrenia. Hove, UK: Erlbaum.

Frith, C., Blackemore, S., & Wolpert, D. (2000). Explaining the symptoms of schizophrenia: Abnormalities in the awareness of action. Brain Research Review, 31(2-3), 357-363.

Gallagher, S., & Zahavi, D. (2008). The phenomenological mind. Londres: Routledge.

Goldie, P. (2011). Anti-empathy. En A. Coplan & P. Goldie (Eds.), Empathy: Philosophical and psychological perspectives (pp. 302-317). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Hatfield, E., Rapson, R. L., & Le, Y. -C. L. (2009). Emotional contagion and empathy. En J. Decety & W. Ickes (Eds.), The social neuroscience of empathy (pp. 19-30). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Hogeveen, J., Obhi, S. S., Banissy, M. J., Santiesteban, I., Press, C., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2015). Task-dependent and distinct roles of the temporoparietal junction and inferior cortex in the control of imitation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(7), 1003-1009. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu148

Joseph, R. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). The relationship of theory of mind and executive functions to symptom type severity in children with autism. Development and Psychopathology, 16, 137-155. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457940404444X

Jurado, M. B., & Roselli, M. (2007). The elusive nature of executive functions: A review of our current understanding. Neuropsychological Review, 17, 213-233. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-007-9040-z

Kluwe-Schiavon, B., Sanvicente-Vieira, B., Kristensen, C. H., & Grassi-Oliveira, R. (2013). Executive functions rehabilitation for schizophrenia: A critical systematic review. Journal of Psichiatric Research, 47, 91-104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.10.001

Lagarde, J., Hantkie, O., Hajjiaoui, A., & Yelnik, A. (2009). Neuropsychological disorders induced by cerebellar damage. Annals of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine, 52(4), 360-370. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rehab.2009.02.002

Letho, J. E., Juujärvi, P., Kooistra, L., & Pulkkinen, L. (2003). Dimentions of executive functioning: evidence from children. Britihs Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 59-80. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151003321164627

Lezak, M. D. (2004). Neuropsychological assesment. Nueva York: Oxford University Press.

López-Silva, P. (2016). The Unity of Consciousness in Pre-Psychotic States. A Phenomenological Analysis. Studies in Psychology, 37(1), 1-34. https://doi.org/10.1080/02109395.2015.1122434

Luria, A. R. (1980). Higher cortical functions in man (2a. ed.). Nueva York: Basic.

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unitiy and diversity of executive function and their contribution to complex “frontal lobe” task: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49-100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

Monette, S., Bigras, M., & Lafrenière, M. A. (2015). Structure of executive functions in typically developing kindergarteners. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 140, 120-139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.005

Pnevmatikos, D., & Trikkaliotis, I. (2013). Intraindividual differences in executive functions during childhood. The role of emotions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115, 245-261. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.01.010

Praetorious, N. (2009). The phenomenological underpinning of the notion of a minimal core self: A psychological perspective. Consciousness and Cognition, 18(1), 325-338. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.005

Press, C., Heyes, C., & Kilner, J. M. (2011). Learning to understand other’s actions. Biology Letters, 7(3), 457-460. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2010.0850

Press, C., Richardson, D., & Bird, G. (2010). Intact imitation of emotional facial actions in autism spectrum conditions. Neuropsychología, 48, 3291-3297. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.07.012

Rapson, R. L., Hatfield, E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1994). Emotional contagion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Rizzolatti, G., & Sinigaglia, C. (2013). Las neuronas espejo. Los mecanismos de la empatía emocional (3.a ed.). Barcelona: Espasa.

Rizzolatti, G. & Sinigaglia, C. (2010). The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 264-274. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2805

Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., & Fogassi, L. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor action. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 3(2), 131-141.

Séguin, J., & Zelazo, P. (2005). Executive function in early physical aggression. En R. E. Tremblay, W. W. Hartup, & J. Archer (Eds.), Developmental Origins of Aggression (pp. 307-329). Nueva York: Guilford.

Spence, S. A., Brooks, D. J., Hirsch, S. R., Liddle, P. F., Meehan, J., & Grasby, P. M. (1997). A PET study of voluntary movement in schizophrenic patients experiencing passivity phenomena (delusions of alien control). Brain. A Journal of Neurology, 120, 1997-2011. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/120.11.1997

Smith, J. (2015). What is empathy for? Synthese, 194(3), 709-722. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-015-0771-8

Sastre-Riba, S., Fonseca-Pedrero, E., & Poch-Olivé, M. L. (2015). Early development of executive functions: A differential study. Anales de Psicología, 31(2), 607-614. https://doi.org/10.6018/analesps.31.2.180711

Shoemaker, S. (1996). The first-person perspective and other essays. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Strawson, G. (1997). The self. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 4(5-6), 405-428.

Tirapu-Ustárroz, J., Pérez-Sayes, G., Erekatxo-Bilbao, M., & Pelegrín-Valero, C. (2007). ¿Qué es la teoría de la mente? Neurología, 44(8), 479-489.

Tirapu-Ustárroz, J., Muñoz-Céspedes, J. M., & Pelegrín-Valero, C. (2003). Hacia una taxonomía de la conciencia. Neurología, 36(11), 1083-1093.

Tomasello, M., & Rakoczy, H. (2003). What makes human cognition unique? From individual to share to collective intentionality. Mind & Language, 18(2), 121-147.

Van Baaren, R., Decety, J., Dijksterhuis, A., Van der Leij, A., & Van Leeuwen, M. L. (2009). Being imitated: Consequences of nonconsciously showing empathy. En J. Decety & W. Ickes (Eds.), The social neuroscience of empathy (pp. 31-42). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Varela, F., & Shear, J. (1999). First person methologies: What, Why, How? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6(2-3), 1-14.

Verdejo-García, A., & Bechara, A. (2010). Neuropsicología de las funciones ejecutivas. Psicothema, 22(2), 227-235.

Waltz, J. (2016). The neural underpinnings of cognitive flexibility and their disruption in psychotic illness. Neuroscience. Publicación anticipada en línea. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.06.005

Yeh, Z. T., Tsai, M. C., Tsai, M. D., Lo, C. Y., & Wang, K. C. (2016). The relationship between theory of mind and the executive functions: Evidence from patients with frontal lobe damage. Applied Neuropsychology. Adult, 9, 1-8. https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2016.1185425

Zahavi, D. (2005). Subjectivity and Selfhood: Investigating the first-person perspective. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

Zahavi, D. (2011). Empathy and direct social perception: A phenomenological proposal. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 2(3), 541-558. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-011-0070-3

Zelazo, P., Qu, L., & Müller, U. (2004). Hot and cool aspects of executive function: relations in early Development. En W. Schneider, R. Schumann & B. Sodian (Eds.), Young children’s cognitive development: Interrelationships among executive junctioning, working memory, verbal ability, and theory of mind (pp. 71-93). Mahwah: Eribaum.

This journal is registered under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International Public License. Thus, this work may be reproduced, distributed, and publicly shared in digital format, as long as the names of the authors and Pontificia Universidad Javeriana are acknowledged. Others are allowed to quote, adapt, transform, auto-archive, republish, and create based on this material, for any purpose (even commercial ones), provided the authorship is duly acknowledged, a link to the original work is provided, and it is specified if changes have been made. Pontificia Universidad Javeriana does not hold the rights of published works and the authors are solely responsible for the contents of their works; they keep the moral, intellectual, privacy, and publicity rights. Approving the intervention of the work (review, copy-editing, translation, layout) and the following outreach, are granted through an use license and not through an assignment of rights. This means the journal and Pontificia Universidad Javeriana cannot be held responsible for any ethical malpractice by the authors. As a consequence of the protection granted by the use license, the journal is not required to publish recantations or modify information already published, unless the errata stems from the editorial management process. Publishing contents in this journal does not generate royalties for contributors.