Spatial Navigation in Children in a Circular Maze: The Interaction between different frames of reference
PDF (Spanish)
HTML (Spanish)

Keywords

associative learning
brain development
infant development
geometric module
psychobiology

How to Cite

Spatial Navigation in Children in a Circular Maze: The Interaction between different frames of reference. (2016). Universitas Psychologica, 15(5). https://doi.org/10.11144/Javeriana.upsy15-5.nenl
Almetrics
 
Dimensions
 

Google Scholar
 
Search GoogleScholar

Abstract

Various theories attempt to explain the strategies of navigation used by children under 6 years old, principle amongst which is the use of geometry. The aim of this study was the spatial orientation applied by 3 and 6- year-old children and their use of different geometric frames of reference and the proximal guide. Children use geometry as the theory of the adaptive combination predicts. 20 six-year-old children and 20 threeyear-old children. The experimental study used a circular maze in which the children had to find a hidden object. The children were split into two groups: those who were disoriented with respect to the outer room, and those who were not disoriented. The 3-year-old children need the geometric information of the outer room, the 6-year-old are also able to use the proximal guide and can use the geometry of the experimental enclosure when their learning has made available the geometry of the room. The results support the theory of adaptive combination, rather than the geometric modules. Furthermore, the presence of reliable geometric frames of reference facilitates the use of other types of keys that are not used in their absence.

PDF (Spanish)
HTML (Spanish)

Best, P. J., White, A. M., & Minai, A. (2001). Spatial processing in the brain: The activity of hippocampal place cells. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 459-486.

Biegler, R. & Morris, R. G. M. (1993). Landmark stability is a prerequisite for spatial but not discrimination learning. Nature, 361, 631–633.

Bullens, J., Nardini, M., Doeller, C. F., Braddick, O., Postma, A., & Burgess, N. (2010). The role of landmarks and boundaries in the development of spatial memory. Developmental Science, 13(1), 170–180.

Cheng, K. (1986). A purely geometric module in the rat spatial representation. Cognition, 23, 149-178.

Cheng, K. & Newcombe, N. S. (2005). Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? Squaring theory and evidence. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(1), 1-23.

Cheng, K., Huttenlocher, J., & Newcombe, N. S. (2013). 25 years of research on the use of geometry in spatial reorientation: a current theoretical perspective. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 20(6), 1033-54 doi:10.3758/s13423-013-0416-1

Cheng, K., Shettleworth, S. J., Huttenlocher, J., & Rieser, J. J. (2007). Bayesian integration of spatial information. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 625–637.

Chiandetti, C., Regolin, L., Sovrano, V. A., & Vallortigara, G. (2007). Spatial reorientation: the effects of space size on the encoding of landmark and geometry information. Animal Cognition, 10, 159–168.

Doeller, C. F. & Burgess, N. (2008). Distinct error-correcting and incidental learning of location relative to landmarks and boundaries. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105, 5909–5914.

Doeller, C. F., King, J. A., & Burgess, N. (2008). Parallel striatal and hippocampal systems for landmarks and boundaries in spatial memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105, 5915–5920.

Hermer, L. & Spelke, E. S. (1994). A Geometric Process for Spatial Reorientation in Young Children. Nature, 370, 77-79.

Hermer-Vazquez, L., Moffet, A. & Munkholm P. (2001). Language, space, and the development of cognitive flexibility in humans: the case of two spatial memory tasks. Cognition, 79(3), 263-99.

Hupbach, A. & Nadel, L. (2005). Reorientation in a rhombic environment: no evidence for an encapsulated geometric module. Cognitive Development, 20, 279–302.

Kretschmann, H. J., Kammradt, G., Krauthausen, I., Sauer, B., & Wingert, F., (1986). Growth of the hippocampal formation in man. Bibliotheca Anatomica, 28, 27-52.

Lackner, J. R. & DiZio, P. (2005). Vestibular, proprioceptive, and haptic contributions to spatial orientation. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 115-47.

Learmonth, A. E., Nadel, L., & Newcombe, N. S. (2002). Children´s use of landmarks: implications for modularity theory. Psychological Science, 13, 337-341.

Learmonth, A., Newcombe, N. S., Sheridan, M., & Jones, M. (2008). Why size counts: Children’s spatial reorientation in large and small enclosures. Developmental Science, 11, 414–426.

Learmonth, A. E., Newcombe. N. S., & Huttenlocher, J. (2001). Toddlers´ use of metric information and landmarks to reorient. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80, 225-244.

Lee, S. A., Sovrano, V. A., & Spelke, E. S. (2012). Navigation as a source of geometric knowledge: Young children’s use of length, angle, distance, and direction in a reorientation task. Cognition, 123, 144-161.

Lee, S. A., & Spelke, E. S. (2010). Two systems of spatial representation underlying navigation. Experimental Brain Research, 206, 179–188.

Lee, S. A., & Spelke, E. S. (2011). Young children reorient by computing layout geometry, not by matching images of the environment. Psychological Bulletin and Review, 18, 192–198.

Lew, A. R., Foster, K. A., & Bremner, J. G. (2006). Disorientation inhibits landmark use in 12–18-month-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 29, 334-341.

Lew, A. R., Gibbons, B., Murphy, C., & Bremner, J. G. (2010). Use of geometry for spatial reorientation in children applies only to symmetric spaces. Developmental Science 13(3), 490–498.

Lourenco, S. F., Addy, D., & Huttenlocher, J. (2008). Location representation in enclosed spaces: What types of information afford young children an advantage? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 313-325.

Lourenco, S. F. & Huttenlocher, J. (2008). The representation of geometric cues in infancy. Infancy, 13(2), 103-127.

Moraleda, E., Broglio, C., Rodríguez, F., & Gómez A. (2013). Development of different spatial frames of reference for orientation in small-scale environments. Psicothema, 25(4), 468-475.

Mrzljak, L., Uilings, H., Van Eden, C., & Judas, M. (1990). Neuronal development in human prefrontal cortex in prenatal and postnatal stages. Progress in Brain Research, 85, 185-222.

Nardini, M., Atkinson, J., & Burgess, N. (2008). Children reorient using the left/right sense of colored landmarks at 18–24 months. Cognition, 106, 519-527.

Nardini, M., Burgess, N., Breckenridge, K., & Atkinson, J. (2006). Differential developmental trajectories for egocentric, environmental and intrinsic frames of reference in spatial memory. Cognition, 101, 153-172.

O’Keefe, J. (2007). Hippocampal neurophysiology in the behaving animal. En P. Andersen, R. Morris, D. Amaral, T. Bliss & J. O’Keefe (Eds.), The hippocampus book (pp. 475-548). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

O´Keefe, J. & Nadel, L. (1978). The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Overman, W. H., Pate, B. J., Moore, K., & Peuster, A. (1996). Ontogeny of place learning in children as measured in the radial arm maze, Morris search task and open field task. Behavioral Neuroscience, 110(6), 1205-28.

Ratliff, K. R. & Newcombe, N. S. (2008). Reorienting when cues conflict: Evidence for an adaptive-combination view. Psychological Science, 19, 1301–1307.

Rescorla, R. A. & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcment and nonreinforcement. En A. H. Black & W. F. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning II: Current theory and research (pp. 64–99). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Ruggiero, G., D'Errico, O., & Iachini, T. (2015). Development of egocentric and allocentric spatial representations from childhood to elderly age. Psychological Research, 80(2), 259-272.

Seress, L. (1992). Morphological variability and developmental aspects of monkey and human granule cells: differences between the rodent and primate dentate gyrus Epilepsy Res. Suppl. 7. En C. E. Ribak (Ed.), The dentate gyrus and its role in seizures (pp. 3-28). Amsterdan: Elsevier Science Publishers B.V.

Sluzenski, J., Newcombe, N., & Ottinger, W. (2004). Changes in reality monitoring and episodic memory in early childhood. Developmental Science, 7(2), 225-245.

Smith, A. D., Gilchrist, I. D., Cater, K., Ikram, N., Nott, K., & Hood, B. M. (2008). Reorientation in the real world: The development of landmark use and integration in a natural environment. Cognition, 107, 1102–1111.

Sowell, E., Thomsom, P., Holmes, C., Batth, R., Jernigan, T., & Toga, A. (1999). Localizing age-related changes in brain structure between childhood and adolescence using statistical parametric mapping. Neuroimage, 9, 587-597.

Taube, J. S. (2011). Head direction cell firing properties and behavioral performance in 3-D space. Journal of Physiology, 589(4), 835-841.

Twyman, A., Friedman, A., & Spetch, M. L. (2007). Penetrating the geometric module: Catalyzing children’s use of landmarks. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1523–1530.

Welberg, L. (2012). Spatial processing: Parietal enthorhinal cortex cells in navigation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 223.

White, N. R. & McDonald, R. J. (2002). Multiple parallel memory systems in the brain of the rat. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 77, 125–184.

This journal is registered under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International Public License. Thus, this work may be reproduced, distributed, and publicly shared in digital format, as long as the names of the authors and Pontificia Universidad Javeriana are acknowledged. Others are allowed to quote, adapt, transform, auto-archive, republish, and create based on this material, for any purpose (even commercial ones), provided the authorship is duly acknowledged, a link to the original work is provided, and it is specified if changes have been made. Pontificia Universidad Javeriana does not hold the rights of published works and the authors are solely responsible for the contents of their works; they keep the moral, intellectual, privacy, and publicity rights. Approving the intervention of the work (review, copy-editing, translation, layout) and the following outreach, are granted through an use license and not through an assignment of rights. This means the journal and Pontificia Universidad Javeriana cannot be held responsible for any ethical malpractice by the authors. As a consequence of the protection granted by the use license, the journal is not required to publish recantations or modify information already published, unless the errata stems from the editorial management process. Publishing contents in this journal does not generate royalties for contributors.