Visual Memory Test based on Snodgrass Pictures (VMT-SP): a New Neuropsychological Measure of Visual Memory on Children with Learning Disabilities
HTML Full Text
PDF
XML

Keywords

visual memory
learning disabilities
neuropsychology
children

How to Cite

Visual Memory Test based on Snodgrass Pictures (VMT-SP): a New Neuropsychological Measure of Visual Memory on Children with Learning Disabilities. (2019). Universitas Psychologica, 18(2), 1-15. https://doi.org/10.11144/Javeriana.upsy18-2.vmtb
Almetrics
 
Dimensions
 

Google Scholar
 
Search GoogleScholar

Abstract

Visual memory can be defined as the ability to recall visual images in the form of objects events or words. Previous neuropsychological research on Learning Disabilities (LD) involving visual memory has been focused particularly on children who present nonverbal LD, and their scholar underachievement. Objective: This study aims to develop a visual memory test using recognisable objects and to determine their normative values and validity in a population of children with LD and in a control group. Methods and procedures: A total of 330 children participated in this study (7-14 years), 190 suffered of some kind of LD and 140 did not have any diagnosis. Visual Memory was assessed using a test (VMT-SP) based on Snodgrass Pictures (Snodgrass & Vanderwart, 1980) composed of a short-term, long-term and a recognition assessment. Rey Complex Figure Test (RCFT) and Hooper Visual Organisation Test (HVOT) were used to assess validity. Results: Short-term, long-term recall and long-term recognition differed significantly between the LD children and the control group. Moreover, the scores were influenced by the age of the children (with a higher percentage of correct answers being given by the older children), gender, habitat and mother’s level of education. Finally, the test results were associated with other measures of visual memory. Conclusions: VMT-SP is a new clinical tool for assessing visual memory. We present evidence of its concurrent validity and applicability in the evaluation of children with LD.

HTML Full Text
PDF
XML

Alloway, T. P., Banner, G. E., & Smith, P. (2010). Working memory and cognitive styles in adolescents’ attainment. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(4), 567-581.

Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. (2001). ASEBA school-age forms & profiles. Burlington: Aseba.

Bain, S. K., & Jaspers, K. E. (2010). Test Review: Review of Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test: Kaufman, AS, & Kaufman, NL (2004). Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test, Second Edition. Bloomington, MN: Pearson, Inc. Journal of Psychoeducational assessment, 28(2), 167-174. https://doi.org/10.1177/0734282909348217

Bird, C. M., & Burgess, N. (2008). The hippocampus and memory: insights from spatial processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(3), 182-194. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2335

Brandenburg, J., Klesczewski, J., Fischbach, A., Schuchardt, K., Büttner, G., & Hasselhorn, M. (2015). Working memory in children with learning disabilities in reading versus spelling: Searching for overlapping and specific cognitive factors. Journal of learning disabilities, 48(6), 622-634. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219414521665

Brankaer, C., Ghesquiere, P., & De Smedt, B. (2014). Numerical magnitude processing deficits in children with mathematical difficulties are independent of intelligence. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35(11), 2603-26261. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2014.06.022

Brown, F. C., Roth, R. M., Saykin, A. J., & Beverly-Gibson, G. (2007). A new measure of visual location learning and memory: development and psychometric properties for the Brown Location Test (BLT). The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 21(5), 811-825. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854040600878777

Bull, R., Espy, K. A., & Wiebe, S. A. (2008). Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in preschoolers: Longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age 7 years. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33(3), 205-228. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565640801982312

Burneo-Garcés, C., Cruz-Quintana, F., Pérez-García, M., Fernández-Alcántara, M., Fasfous, A., & Pérez-Marfil, M. N. (2019). Interaction between Socioeconomic Status and Cognitive Development in Children Aged 7, 9, and 11 Years: A Cross-Sectional Study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 44(1), 1-16. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2018.1554662

Correa, C., Fernández-Alcántara, M., Pérez-García, M., Laynez-Rubio, C., & Cruz-Quintana, F. (2017). Effects of an Executive Functions stimulation programme for children with learning disabilities/Efectos de un programa de estimulación de las Funciones Ejecutivas en niños con dificultades de aprendizaje. Estudios de Psicología, 38(2), 537-551. https://doi.org/10.1080/02109395.2017.1295576

Corsi, P. M. (1973). Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). McGill University. Montreal. Retrieved from http://digitool.library.mcgill.ca/webclient/StreamGate?folder_id=0&dvs=1558916387370~169

Duncan, G. J., & Magnuson, K. (2012). Socioeconomic status and cognitive functioning: moving from correlation to causation. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 3(3), 377-386. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1176

Cuetos, F., Ellis, A. W., & Alvarez, B. (1999). Naming times for the Snodgrass and Vanderwart pictures in Spanish. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 31(4), 650-658. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03200741

Eng, H. Y., Chen, D., & Jiang, Y. (2005). Visual working memory for simple and complex visual stimuli. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(6), 1127-1133. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206454

Freire, C., Ramos, R., Lopez-Espinosa, M.-J., Díez, S., Vioque, J., Ballester, F., & Fernández, M.-F. (2010). Hair mercury levels, fish consumption, and cognitive development in preschool children from Granada, Spain. Environmental Research, 110(1), 96-104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envres.2009.10.005

Garcia, R. B., Mammarella, I. C., Tripodi, D., & Cornoldi, C. (2014). Visuospatial working memory for locations, colours, and binding in typically developing children and in children with dyslexia and non‐verbal learning disability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 32(1), 17-33. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12019

Garcia, R. B., Mammarella, I. C., Pancera, A., Galera, C., & Cornoldi, C. (2015). Deficits in visual short-term memory binding in children at risk of non-verbal learning disabilities. Research in developmental disabilities, 45-46, 365-372. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2015.07.035

Hooper, E. H. (1983). Hooper visual organization test (VOT). Los Angeles, LA: Western Psychological Services.

Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2004). Kaufman brief intelligence test. John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Klesczewski, J., Brandenburg, J., Fischbach, A., Grube, D., Hasselhorn, M., & Büttner, G. (2015). Working memory functioning in children with poor mathematical skills. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 233, 83-92. https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000206

Liebel, S. W., & Nelson, J. M. (2017). Auditory and Visual Working memory functioning in college students with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and/or Learning Disabilities. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 32(8), 980-991. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acx014

Lowe, P. A., Mayfield, J. W., & Reynolds, C. R. (2003). Gender differences in memory test performance among children and adolescents. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 18(8), 865-878. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0887-6177(02)00162-2

Luck, S. J., & Hollingworth, A. R. (2008). Visual memory. New York, NY: University Press.

Maehler, C., & Schuchardt, K. (2016). The importance of working memory for school achievement in primary school children with intellectual or learning disabilities. Research in developmental disabilities, 58, 1-8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2016.08.007

Mammarella, I. C., & Pazzaglia, F. (2010). Visual perception and memory impairments in children at risk of nonverbal learning disabilities. Child Neuropsychology, 16(6), 564-576. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2010.485125

Mammarella, I. C., Giofrè, D., Ferrara, R., & Cornoldi, C. (2013). Intuitive geometry and visuospatial working memory in children showing symptoms of nonverbal learning disabilities. Child Neuropsychology, 19(3), 235-249. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2011.640931

McGivern, R. F., Mutter, K. L., Anderson, J., Wideman, G., Bodnar, M., & Huston, P. J. (1998). Gender differences in incidental learning and visual recognition memory: support for a sex difference in unconscious environmental awareness. Personality and Individual Differences, 25(2), 223-232. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00017-8

Montgomery, J. W. (2000). Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43(2), 293-308. https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4302.293

Mous, S. E., Schoemaker, N. K., Blanken, L. M., Thijssen, S., van der Ende, J., Polderman, T. J., … White, T. (2017). The association of gender, age, and intelligence with neuropsychological functioning in young typically developing children: The generation R study. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 6(1), 22-40. https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2015.1067214

Müller, B. W., Gimbel, K., Keller-Pließnig, A., Sartory, G., Gastpar, M., & Davids, E. (2007). Neuropsychological assessment of adult patients with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 257(2), 112-119. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-006-0688-9

Muñoz-Vinuesa, A., Afonso-Martín, A., Cruz-Quintana, F., Pérez-Marfil, M. N., Sotomayor-Morales, E. M., & Fernández-Alcántara, M. (2018). Determinantes sociales de la salud. Estatus socioeconómico, neurodesarrollo y funciones ejecutivas en la infancia. Index Enfermería, 27, 143-146. Retrieved from https://rua.ua.es/dspace/bitstream/10045/85571/1/2018_Munoz-Vinuesa_etal_IndexEnferm.pdf

Narimoto, T., Matsuura, N., Takezawa, T., Mitsuhashi, Y., & Hiratani, M. (2013). Spatial short-term memory in children with nonverbal learning disabilities: Impairment in encoding spatial configuration. The Journal of genetic psychology, 174(1), 73-87. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325.2011.641040

Noble, K. G., Houston, S. M., Brito, N. H., Bartsch, H., Kan, E., Kuperman, J. M., ... & Schork, N. J. (2015). Family income, parental education and brain structure in children and adolescents. Nature neuroscience, 18(5), 773. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3983

Passolunghi, M. C. (2006). Working memory and mathematical disability. In T. Packiam Alloway & S. Gathercole (Eds.), Working memory and neurodevelopmental condition (pp. 113–138). Hove, UK: Psychology Press

Pauls, F., Petermann, F., & Lepach, A. C. (2013). Gender differences in episodic memory and visual working memory including the effects of age. Memory, 21(7), 857-874. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2013.765892

Pérez, M. A. & Navalón, C. (2003). Normas españolas de 290 nuevos dibujos: Acuerdo en la denominación, concordancia de la imagen, familiaridad, complejidad visual y variabilidad de la imagen. Psicológica: Revista de Metodología y Psicología Learning Disability, 24(2), 215-242.

Peterson, D. J., & Berryhill, M. E. (2013). The Gestalt principle of similarity benefits visual working memory. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 20(6), 1282-1289. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0460-x

Pissella, L., & Mattingley, J. B. (2004). The contribution of spatial remapping impairments to unilateral visual neglect. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 28(2), 181-200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.03.003

Price, J. P. (2009). The computerized object and abstract designs test (COAD): A pilot study of a new test of visual working memory. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 48(2), 109-123. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466508X366713

Putzke, J. D., Williams, M. A., Adams, W., & Boll, T. J. (1998). Does memory test performance in children become more consistent with age? Cross-sectional comparisons using the WRAML. Journal of Clinical and Learning Disability Neuropsychology, 20(6), 835-845. Retrieved from https://eurekamag.com/research/010/491/010491425.php

Reales, J. M., Ballesteros, S., & García, E. (2002). Visual-word identification thresholds for the 260 fragmented words of the Snodgrass and Vanderwart pictures in Spanish. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 34(2), 276-285. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195455

Rey, A. (1980). REY. Test de Copia de una Figura Compleja. Madrid: Tea Ediciones

Rhodes, S. M., Coghill, D. R., & Matthews, K. (2004). Methylphenidate restores visual memory, but not working memory function in attention deficit-hyperkinetic disorder. Psychopharmacology, 175(3), 319-330. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-004-1833-7

Robert, M., & Savoie, N. (2006). Are there gender differences in verbal and visuospatial working-memory resources? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 18(3), 378-397. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440500234104

Santos, F. H., Mello, C. B., Bueno, O. F. A., & Dellatolas, G. (2005). Cross-cultural differences for three visual memory tasks in Brazilian children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 101(2), 421-433. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.101.2.421-433

Schuchardt, K., & Mähler, C. (2016). Exekutive Funktionen bei Kindern mit Lernstörungen. Praxis der Kinderpsychologie und Kinderpsychiatrie, 65(6), 389-405. https://doi.org/10.13109/prkk.2016.65.6.389

Seigneuric, A., Ehrlich, M.-F., Oakhill, J. V., & Yuill, N. M. (2000). Working memory resources and children’s reading comprehension. Reading and Writing, 13(1-2), 81-103. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1008088230941

Semrud-Clikeman, M., Walkowiak, J., Wilkinson, A., & Butcher, B. (2010). Executive functioning in children with Asperger syndrome, ADHD-combined type, ADHD-predominantly inattentive type, and controls. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(8), 1017-1027. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.04.281

Shang, C. Y., & Gau, S. S. (2011). Visual memory as a potential cognitive endophenotype of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychological Medicine, 41(12), 2603-2614. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291711000857

Snodgrass, J. G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Learning Disability Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6(2), 174. Retrieved from https://lslab.lscore.ucla.edu/MIT/MIT_Papers/PMIT%20260%20Snodgrass%201980.pdf

Soprano, A. M. (2003). Evaluación de las funciones ejecutivas en el niño. Revista de Neurología, 37(1), 44-50. https://doi.org/10.33588/rn.3701.2003237

Stadthagen-González, H., Damian, M. F., Pérez, M. A., Bowers, J. S., & Marín, J. (2009). Name-picture verification as a control measure for object naming: A task analysis and norms for a large set of pictures. The Quarterly Journal of Learning Disability Psychology, 62(8), 1581-1597. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210802511139

Stevenson, H. W., Chen, C., & Booth, J. (1990). Influences of schooling and urban-rural residence on gender differences in cognitive abilities and academic achievement. Sex Roles, 23(9-10), 535-551. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00289767

Tirado-Durán, E. (2006). Evaluación neuropsicológica y neurocirugía psiquiátrica. Salud Mental, 29, 13-17. Retrieved from: https://www.medigraphic.com/cgi-bin/new/resumen.cgi?IDARTICULO=13932

Trahan, D. E., & Quintana, J. W. (1990). Analysis of gender effects upon verbal and visual memory performance in adults. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 5(4), 325-334. https://doi.org/10.1016/0887-6177(90)90012-E

Wang, L., Chen, C.W., & Zhu, L. (2014). Picture norms for Chinese preschool children: Name agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. PloS One, 9(3), e90450. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0090450

Werpup-Stuewe, L., & Petermann, F. (2015). Visual perception abilities in children with reading disabilities. Zeitschrift fur Kinder-und Jugendpsychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 43(3), 195-205. https://doi.org/10.1024/1422-4917/a000353

Willis, S., Goldbart, J., & Stansfield, J. (2014). The strengths and weaknesses in verbal short-term memory and visual working memory in children with hearing impairment and additional language learning difficulties. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 78(7), 1107-1114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2014.04.025

Wyatt, B. S., Conners, F. A., & Carr, M. D. (1998). The Snodgrass picture fragment completion test: Alternate-form reliability. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 30(2), 360-368.

This journal is registered under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International Public License. Thus, this work may be reproduced, distributed, and publicly shared in digital format, as long as the names of the authors and Pontificia Universidad Javeriana are acknowledged. Others are allowed to quote, adapt, transform, auto-archive, republish, and create based on this material, for any purpose (even commercial ones), provided the authorship is duly acknowledged, a link to the original work is provided, and it is specified if changes have been made. Pontificia Universidad Javeriana does not hold the rights of published works and the authors are solely responsible for the contents of their works; they keep the moral, intellectual, privacy, and publicity rights. Approving the intervention of the work (review, copy-editing, translation, layout) and the following outreach, are granted through an use license and not through an assignment of rights. This means the journal and Pontificia Universidad Javeriana cannot be held responsible for any ethical malpractice by the authors. As a consequence of the protection granted by the use license, the journal is not required to publish recantations or modify information already published, unless the errata stems from the editorial management process. Publishing contents in this journal does not generate royalties for contributors.