Modulation of Physiological Response to Affective Stimuli: A Tool for Psychological Processes Assessment
HTML Full Text (Spanish)
PDF (Spanish)
XML (Spanish)

Keywords

orientation
attention
emotion
motivation
psychophysiology

How to Cite

Modulation of Physiological Response to Affective Stimuli: A Tool for Psychological Processes Assessment. (2019). Universitas Psychologica, 18(1), 1-12. https://doi.org/10.11144/Javeriana.upsy18-1.mrfe
Almetrics
 
Dimensions
 

Google Scholar
 
Search GoogleScholar

Abstract

Conventional tools for the assessment of psychological processes rely on the use of self-report questionnaires and behavioral tasks, in spite of the existence of objective and more accurate physiological responses that could provide new insights into such processes. Accordingly, the aim of this study was to analyze the changes in different physiological responses to stimuli with different affective content, to obtain physiological indicators of the attentional, emotional and motivational processes. The responses of 40 participants were recorded while observing pleasant, neutral and unpleasant stimuli. Results show that the pleasant and unpleasant stimuli generate a greater galvanic skin response, which indicates a greater sympathetic activation and a greater orientation response, as well as a greater heart rate deceleration and greater prepulse inhibition, which suggests a greater attentional response. It was also found that unpleasant stimuli enhance the startle reflex and the pleasant stimuli inhibit it, making it a physiological marker of motivational response. There were no significant differences between men and women. Overall, the results support the use of these methodologies for the study of orientation/attention, emotion and motivation.

HTML Full Text (Spanish)
PDF (Spanish)
XML (Spanish)

Blumenthal, T. D., Cuthbert, B. N., Filion, D. L., Hackley, S., Lipp, O. V., & van Boxtel, A. (2005). Committee report: Guidelines for human startle eyeblink electromyographic studies. Psychophysiology, 42, 1–15.

Bradley, M. (2009). Natural selective attention: Orienting and emotion. Psychophysiology, 46, 1–11. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00702.x.

Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B., & Lang, P. J. (2001). Emotion and motivation I: Defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion, 1(3), 276 – 298. doi: 10.1037//1528-3542.1.3.276.

Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., & Lang, P. J. (2006). A multi‐process account of startle modulation during affective perception. Psychophysiology, 43(5), 486-497. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00412.x.

Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Sabatinelli, D., & Lang, P. J. (2001). Emotion and motivation II: Sex differences in picture processing. Emotion, 1(3), 300-319. doi: 10.1037//1528-3542.1.3.300

Bradley, M. M., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (1993). Pictures as prepulse: Attention and emotion in startle modification. Psychophysiology, 30(5), 541-545.

Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25, 49–59.

Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2007). Emotion and motivation. In: The Handbook of Psychophysiology. Cacioppo, J., Tassinary, L., & Berntson, G. (Eds.). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Duval, E. R., Lovelace, C. T., Aarant, J., & Filion, D. L. (2013). The time course of face processing: Startle eyeblink response modulation by face gender and expression. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 90(3), 354-357. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.08.006.

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146t.

Gantiva, C., Camacho, K., & Betancourt-Piedrahita, M. (2016). Características de la respuesta emocional generada por las palabras: Un estudio experimental desde la emoción y la motivación. Psychologia: Avances de la Disciplina, 10(2), 69-76.

Gantiva, C., Delgado, R., & Romo-González, T. (2015). Emotional reactions to alcohol-related words: Differences between low- and high-risk drinkers. Addictive Behaviors 50, 60–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.06.004.

Gantiva, C., Estupiñan, E., Montaña, I., Sierra, M., Zocadegui, E., & Romo-González, T. (2014). Emotional dimensions in people with aggressive behavior: differential responses to affective visual stimuli. Trends in Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, 36(4), 203-208. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2237-6089-2014-0004.

Gantiva, C., Guerra, P., & Vila, J. (2011). Validación colombiana del sistema internacional de imágenes afectivas: evidencias del origen transcultural de la emoción. Acta Colombiana de Psicología, 14(2), 103-111.

Gantiva, C., Guerra, P., & Vila, J. (2015). Modulación del reflejo de sobresalto en población colombiana: evidencia de la interacción entre emoción y motivación. Universitas Psychologica, 14(1), 157-164. http://dx.doi.org/10.11144/Javeriana.upsy14-1.mrsp.

Gantiva, C., Rodríguez, M., Arias, M., & Rubio, E. (2012). Dimensiones de la emoción durante el proceso de abandono del consumo de tabaco: un apoyo a la visión motivacional del cambio. Diversitas, 8(2), 309-317.

Gard, D. E., Gard, M. G., Mehta, N., Kring, A. M., & Patrick, C. J. (2007). Impact of motivational salience on affect modulated startle at early and late probe times. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 66(3), 266-270. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.05.005.

Globisch, J., Hamm, A. O., Esteves, F., & Öhman, A. (1999). Fear appears fast: Temporal course of startle reflex potentiation in animal fearful subjects. Psychophysiology, 36(1), 66-75.

Graham, F. K., & Clifton, R. K. (1966). Heart-rate change as a component of the orienting response. Psychological Bulletin, 65(5), 305-320.

Grillon, C., & Baas, J. (2003). A review of the modulation of the startle reflex by affective states and its application in psychiatry. Clinical neurophysiology, 114(9), 1557-1579.

Hamm, A. O., Cuthbert, B. N., Globisch, J., & Vaitl, D. (1997). Fear and the startle reflex: Blink modulation and autonomic response patterns in animal and mutilation fearful subjects. Psychophysiology, 34(1), 97-107.

Lang, P. J. (1995). The emotion probe: Studies of motivation and attention. American Psychologist. 50, 372-385.

Lang, P.J., Bradley, M., & Cuthbert, B. (2008). International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report A-8. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.

Lang, P. J., Davis, M., & Öhman, A. (2000). Fear and anxiety: animal models and human cognitive psychophysiology. Journal of Affective Disorders, 61(3), 137-159.

Lang, P. J., Simons, R. F., & Balaban, M. (2013). Attention and orienting: Sensory and motivational processes. New Jersey: Psychology Press.

Miccoli, L., Delgado, R., Rodríguez-Ruiz, S., Guerra, P., García-Mármol, E., & Fernández-Santaella, M. C. (2014). Meet OLAF, a good friend of the IAPS! The Open Library of Affective Foods: a tool to investigate the emotional impact of food in adolescents. PloS one, 9(12), e114515.

Muñoz, M., Viedma-del-Jesús, M., Rosselló, F., Sánchez-Nácher, N., Montoya, P., & Vila, J. (2013). The emotional impact of European tobacco-warning images. Tobacco Control, 22, 123-129. doi:10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2011-050070.

Muñoz, M., Idrissi, S., Sánchez-Barrera, M., Fernández, M., & Vila, J. (2011). Motivation to quit smoking and startle modulation in female smokers: context specificity of smoking cue reactivity. Psychopharmacology, 218, 525-532. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2334-0.

Richardson, J. T. (2011). Eta squared and partial eta squared as measures of effect size in educational research. Educational Research Review, 6(2), 135-147. doi:10.1016/j.edurev.2010.12.001.

This journal is registered under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International Public License. Thus, this work may be reproduced, distributed, and publicly shared in digital format, as long as the names of the authors and Pontificia Universidad Javeriana are acknowledged. Others are allowed to quote, adapt, transform, auto-archive, republish, and create based on this material, for any purpose (even commercial ones), provided the authorship is duly acknowledged, a link to the original work is provided, and it is specified if changes have been made. Pontificia Universidad Javeriana does not hold the rights of published works and the authors are solely responsible for the contents of their works; they keep the moral, intellectual, privacy, and publicity rights. Approving the intervention of the work (review, copy-editing, translation, layout) and the following outreach, are granted through an use license and not through an assignment of rights. This means the journal and Pontificia Universidad Javeriana cannot be held responsible for any ethical malpractice by the authors. As a consequence of the protection granted by the use license, the journal is not required to publish recantations or modify information already published, unless the errata stems from the editorial management process. Publishing contents in this journal does not generate royalties for contributors.