Contextual Renewal of Temporal Patterns of Responding
HTML Full Text (Spanish)
PDF (Spanish)
XML (Spanish)

Keywords

acquisition
extinction
contextual renewal
fixed interval
rats

How to Cite

Contextual Renewal of Temporal Patterns of Responding. (2018). Universitas Psychologica, 17(3), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.11144/Javeriana.upsy17-3.rcdt
Almetrics
 
Dimensions
 

Google Scholar
 
Search GoogleScholar

Abstract

It has been reported that in certain phenomena (spontaneous recovery, reacquisition, resurgence and contextual renewal) it is possible to observe the recovery of a previously extinguished response. Contextual renewal refers to the reappearance of an extinguished response that result from a change of context between the extinction and testing phase. The purpose of the present study was to evaluate contextual renewal, considering both the temporal distribution of the responses and the response rate. A recovery of the response rate was found, being a result consistent with those reported in the area of ​​contextual renewal. In addition, a renewal of the temporal distribution of the responses could be identified. The results are discussed by recognizing that rate and temporal distribution are two behavioral dimensions that are modulated by contextual changes.

HTML Full Text (Spanish)
PDF (Spanish)
XML (Spanish)

Bernal-Gamboa, R., Carrasco-López, M. & Nieto, J. (2014). Contrasting ABA, AAB and ABC renewal in a free operant procedure. Spanish Journal of Psychology, 17, 01-06. http://doi.org/10.1017/sjp.2014.68

Bouton, M. & Bolles, R. (1979). Contextual control of the extinction of conditioned fear. Learning and Motivation 10, 445-466.

Bouton, M., Todd, T., Vurbic, D. & Winterbauer, N. (2011). Renewal after the extinction of free operant behavior. Learning & Behavior, 39, 57–67. http://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-011-0018-6

Cabrer, F. (1987). Efectos de la duración de un reloj externo sobre un programa de intervalo fijo. Revista Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta, 13, 153-172.

Cançado, C. & Lattal, K. (2011). Resurgence of temporal patterns of responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 95, 271–287. http://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2011.95-271

Cuevas, K., Learmonth, A. & Roveer-Collier, C. (2015). A dissociation between recognition and reactivation: The renewal effect at 3 months of age. Developmental Psychobiology, 58, 159-175. http://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21357

Delamater, A. (Junio de 2016). On the relative permanence of memories processes involved in the acquisition and extinction of pavlovian learning. Preseminario llevado a cabo en Centro de Estudios e Investigación en Comportamiento, Guadalajara, Jalisco.

Dews, P. (1969). Studies on responding under fixed-interval schedules of reinforcement: the effects on the pattern of responding of changes in requirements at reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 191-199.

Eddy, M., Todd, T., Bouton, M. & Green, J. (2016). Medial prefrontal cortex involvement in the expression of extinction and ABA renewal of instrumental behavior for a food reinforce. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 128, 33-39. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2015.12.003

Ferster, C.B. & Skinner, B.F. (1957). Schedules of Reinforcement. New York: Appleton Century Crofts.

Gentry, G. D., Weiss, B., & Laties, V. G. (1983). The microanalysis of fixed-interval responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 39, 327–343. http://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1983.39-327

Gilbert, T. F. (1958). Fundamental dimensional properties of the operant. Psychological Review, 65(5), 272–282. http://doi.org/10.1037/h0044071

Guilhardi, P., & Church, R. M. (2004). Measures of temporal discrimination in fixed-interval performance: a case study in archiving data. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 661–669. http://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206548

Guilhardi, P., & Church, R. (2006). The pattern of responding after extensive extinction. Learning & Behavior, 34, 269–284. http://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192883

López, F., & Menez, M. (2005). Effects of reinforcement history on response rate and response pattern in periodic reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 83, 221–241. http://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2005.49-04

Kearns, D. & Weiss, S. (2007). Contextual renewal of cocaine seeking in rats and its attenuation by the conditioned effects of an alternative reinforce. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 90, 193-202. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.03.006

Kelley, M., Linddon, C., Ribeiro, A., Greif, A. & Podlesnisk, C. (2015). Basic and translational evaluation of renewal of operant responding. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 48, 390-401. http://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.209

Nakajima, S., Tanaka, S., Urushihara, K., & Imada, H. (2000). Renewal of extinguished lever-Press responses upon return to the training context. Learning and Motivation, 31, 416–431. http://doi.org/10.1006/lmot.2000.1064

Podlesnik, C. & Miranda-Dukoski, L. (2015). Stimulus generalization and operant context renewal. Behavioural Processes, 119, 93-98. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2015.07.015

Sidman, M. (1960). Tactics of scientific research: Evaluating experimental data in psychology. New York: Basic Books.

Skinner, B. F. (1938). The Behavior of Organisms. New York: Appleton Century Crofts.

Sweeney, M. & Shahan, T. (2015). Renewal, resurgence, and alternative reinforcement context. Behavioural Processes, 116, 43-49. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2015.04.015

Todd, T., Winterbauer, N. & Bouton, M. (2012). Effects of the amount of acquisition and contextual generalization on the renewal of instrumental behavior after extinction. Learning & Behavior, 40, 145-157. http://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-011-0051-5

Trask, S. & Bouton, M. (2016). Discriminative properties of the reinforcer can be used to attenuate the renewal of extinguished operant behavior. Learning & Behavior, 44, 151-161. http://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-015-0195-9

Vurbie, D., Gold, B. & Bouton, M. (2011). Effects of D-Cycloserine on the extinction of appetitive operant learning. Behavioral Neuroscience, 125, 551-559. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0024403

This journal is registered under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International Public License. Thus, this work may be reproduced, distributed, and publicly shared in digital format, as long as the names of the authors and Pontificia Universidad Javeriana are acknowledged. Others are allowed to quote, adapt, transform, auto-archive, republish, and create based on this material, for any purpose (even commercial ones), provided the authorship is duly acknowledged, a link to the original work is provided, and it is specified if changes have been made. Pontificia Universidad Javeriana does not hold the rights of published works and the authors are solely responsible for the contents of their works; they keep the moral, intellectual, privacy, and publicity rights. Approving the intervention of the work (review, copy-editing, translation, layout) and the following outreach, are granted through an use license and not through an assignment of rights. This means the journal and Pontificia Universidad Javeriana cannot be held responsible for any ethical malpractice by the authors. As a consequence of the protection granted by the use license, the journal is not required to publish recantations or modify information already published, unless the errata stems from the editorial management process. Publishing contents in this journal does not generate royalties for contributors.