Effects of Chess Training Method, Comprehensive Training Versus Tactical Training, in Cognitive and Socio-Emotional Competences of Schoolchildren
PDF (Spanish)

Keywords

chess
training method
intellectual enrichment
emotional development
social development
schoolchildren

How to Cite

Aciego, R., García, L., & Betancort, M. (2016). Effects of Chess Training Method, Comprehensive Training Versus Tactical Training, in Cognitive and Socio-Emotional Competences of Schoolchildren. Universitas Psychologica, 15(1), 165-176. https://doi.org/10.11144/Javeriana.upsy15-1.emea
Almetrics
 
Dimensions
 

Google Scholar
 
Search GoogleScholar

Abstract

It is analyzed the effect of chess training using a methodology focused on development of cognitive and socio-emotional competences or focused on tactical training, with a sample of compulsory education. In a quasi-experimental design: chess group 1, focused on comprehensive training (n= 110); chess group 2, focused on tactical training (n= 60); and comparison group, basketball and football activities (n= 60). Dependent variables analyzed, applied at the beginning and end of the academic course, were: cognitive competence, as assessed by performance testing; socio-emotional competence, through self-evaluation test; and chess competence, through performance test. Results showed that comprehensive training group improves not only in basic cognitive skills (attention and memory), as the group 2, but also in more complex cognitive skills (association, analysis and synthesis, planning and forecasting, etc). In socio-emotional level, not only in somatizations and classroom behavior, but also in self-esteem, motivation and attitude toward study. It is concluded that school children who practice chess obtained significantly superior results, in contrast to those who practice other activities (football or basketball), in cognitive and socio-personal skills, especially when a sensible comprehensive training methodology used.

PDF (Spanish)

Aciego, R., García, L., & Betancort, M. (2012). The benefits of chess for the intellectual and socialemotional enrichment in scholchildren. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 15, 551-559. http://dx.doi. org/10.5209/rev_SJOP.2012.v15.n2.38866

Aguilera, R. (1981). Tratado elemental de Ajedrez. Madrid: Fundamentos.

Bilalic´, M., Langner, R., Erb, M., & Groud, W. (2010). Mechanisms and neural basis of object and pattern recognition: A study with chess experts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138, 728-742. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0020756

Bilalic´, M., McLeod, P., & Gobet, F. (2009). Specialization effect and its influence on memory and problem solving in expert chess players. Cognitive Science, 33, 1117–1143. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ j.1551-6709.2009.01030.x

Campitelli, G., Gobet, F., & Parker, A. (2005). Structure and Stimulus Familiarity: A Study of Memory in Chess-Players with Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 8, 238-245.

Castro, P. (2006). Ajedrez infantil. Barcelona: Paidotribo.

Chandler, M. (2005). Tácticas de ajedrez para niños. Madrid: La casa de Ajedrez.

Charness, N. (1998). Perception and memory in chess: “A Royal Wedding?” Review of Adrian D. de Groot and Fernand Gobet Perception and memory in chess. Studies in the heuristics of the professional eye. Contemporary Psychology, 43, 416-417. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1037/001681

Charness, N., Tuffiash, M., & Krampe, R. (2005). The role of deliberate practice in chess expertise. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 151-165. http://dx.doi. org/10.1002/acp.1106

Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). Perception in chess. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 55–81. http://dx.doi. org/10.1016/0010-0285(73)90004-2

Christiaen, J., & Verhofstadt-Denève, L. (1981). Schaken en cognitieve ontwikkeling [Chess and cognitive development]. Nederlands Tijdschrift voor de Psychologie, 36, 561-582.

De Groot, A.D. (1946). Het denken van den Schaker, een experimenteel-psychologische studie [The thinking of the chess player: an experimental - psychological study]. Doctoral Thesis, University of Amsterdam. Amsterdam, Holland. Retrieved from http://www.dbnl.org/tekst/groo004denk01_01/ groo004denk01_01.pdf

De Groot, A.D. (1965). Thought and choice in chess. The Hague, The Netherlands: Mouton & Co Publishers.

Duan, X.; He, Sh.; Liao, W.; Liang, D.; Qiu, L.; Wei, L…. Chen, H. (2012). Reduced caudate volume and enhanced striatal-DMN integration in chess experts. NeuroImage 60, 1280-1286. http://dx.doi. org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.047.

Ferguson, R.C. (1995, January). Chess in education research summary: Paper presented at the Chess in education a wise move conference, Borough of Manhattan Community College, New York. Retrieved from http://www.gardinerchess.com/publications/ ciers.pdf

Fernández-Amigo, J. (2008). Utilización de material didáctico con recursos de ajedrez para la enseñanza de las matemáticas. Estudio de sus efectos sobre una muestra de alumnos de 2º de primaria. Tesis doctoral. Barcelona: Universidad Autónomo de Barcelona. Recuperado de http://www.tesisenred.net/bitstream/handle/10803/5053/jfa1de1. pdf?sequence=1

Frank, A., & D’Hondt, W. (1979). Aptitudes et apprentissage du jeu d’échecs au Zaire [Aptitudes and learning of the game of chess in Zaire]. Psychopathologie Africaine, 15, 81-98.

Gairín, J., & Fernández-Amigo, J. (2010). Enseñar matemáticas con recursos de ajedrez. Tendencias pedagógicas, 15, 57-90.

García, F. (1998). Aportaciones educativas del juego del Ajedrez. Comunicación y pedagogía: Nuevas tecnologías y recursos didácticos, 152, 116-119.

García, F. (2001). Educando desde el ajedrez. Barcelona: Paidotribo.

Gobet, F., & Simon, H. A. (1996). Templates in chess memory: A mechanism for recalling several boards. Cognitive Psychology, 31, 1–40. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1996.0011

Gobet, F., & Simon, H. A. (1998). Expert chess memory: Revisiting the chunking hypothesis. Memory, 6, 225–255. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/741942359

Hayes, S.C., Luoma, J.B., Bond, F., Masuda, A., & Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: Model, processes and outcomes. Behavior Research and Therapy, 44, 1-25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. brat.2005.06.006

Hernández, P. (1983). Test Autoevaluativo Multifactorial de Adaptación Infantil (TAMAI). Madrid: TEA.

Hernández, J.A. & Betancort, M. (s.f.) Paquete de funciones para los análisis ULLRToolbox. Retrieved from https://sites.google.com/site/ullrtoolbox/

Hernández, P., & Rodríguez, H. (2006). Success in chess mediated by mental molds. Psicothema, 18, 704-710.

Kelly, E. J. (1985). The personality of chess players. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 282-284. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4903_13

Krawczyk, D. C., Boggan, A. L., McClelland, M. M., & Bartlett, J. C. (2011). The neural organization of perception in chess experts. Neuroscience Letters, 499, 64-69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2011.05.033

Laplaza, J. (2001). La evaluación en ajedrez. Recuperado de http://www.laplaza.org.ar/pedagogo/evaluacion. htm

Linhares, A. (2005). An active symbols theory of chess intuition. Minds and Machines, 15, 131–181. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11023-005-5045-7

Linhares, A., & Freitas, A. E. (2010). Questioning Chase and Simon’s (1973) “perception in chess”: The “experience recognition” hypothesis. New Ideas in Psychology, 28, 64–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. newideapsych.2009.07.008

Linhares, A., Freitas, A.E., Mendes, A., & Silva, J.S. (2012). Entanglement of perception and reasoning in the combinatorial game of chess: Differential errors of strategic reconstruction. Cognitive Systems Research, 13, 72–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. cogsys.2010.12.006

Liptrap, J.M. (1998). Chess and standard test scores. Chess Life, 41-43.

Margulies, S. (1996). The effect of chess son reading scores: District Nine Chess Program Second. New York. Retrieved from http://www.gardinerchess. com/publications/BenefitsOfChessInEdScreen2 %5B1%5D.pdf

Pallarés, M., & Fernández-Amigo, J. (2004). Estrategias y recursos para una aproximación de la enseñanza del ajedrez a niños con NEE. Comunicación y pedagogía: Nuevas tecnologías y recursos didácticos, 193, 26-33.

Rennig, J., Bilali´c, M., Huberle, E., Karnath, H., & Himmelbach, M. (2013). The temporo-parietal junction contributes to global Gestalt perception — evidence from studies in chess experts. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 513.

Rodríguez, J. (1996). Influencia del ajedrez como actividad excátedra para mejorar el rendimiento académico en alumnos de la segunda etapa de Educación Básica en la Unidad Educativa Estatal “Piloncito”. Tesis doctoral. Caracas. Venezuela: Universidad Nacional Abierta.

Ruiz. F.J. (2006). Application of the therapy of acceptance and commitment (TAC) to increase chess performance. A case study. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 6, 77-97.

Ruiz, F.J., & Luciano, C. (2009). Eficacia de la terapia de aceptación y compromiso (ACT) en la mejora del rendimiento ajedrecístico de jóvenes promesas. Psicothema, 21(3), 347-352.

Salazar, A. (1999). Juega el maestro y ganan los niños. Barcelona: Fundación M. Pilar Mas.

Segura, A. (2002). Enseñanza del ajedrez en primaria. Madrid: Paidotribo.

Todorcevic, M. (2006). Compendio de Ajedrez. Madrid: La Casa de Ajedrez.

Wechsler, D. (1974). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children [traducción al español: Escala de inteligencia para niños de Wechlser, WISC-R. Madrid: TEA, 1993].

This journal is registered under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International Public License. Thus, this work may be reproduced, distributed, and publicly shared in digital format, as long as the names of the authors and Pontificia Universidad Javeriana are acknowledged. Others are allowed to quote, adapt, transform, auto-archive, republish, and create based on this material, for any purpose (even commercial ones), provided the authorship is duly acknowledged, a link to the original work is provided, and it is specified if changes have been made. Pontificia Universidad Javeriana does not hold the rights of published works and the authors are solely responsible for the contents of their works; they keep the moral, intellectual, privacy, and publicity rights. Approving the intervention of the work (review, copy-editing, translation, layout) and the following outreach, are granted through an use license and not through an assignment of rights. This means the journal and Pontificia Universidad Javeriana cannot be held responsible for any ethical malpractice by the authors. As a consequence of the protection granted by the use license, the journal is not required to publish recantations or modify information already published, unless the errata stems from the editorial management process. Publishing contents in this journal does not generate royalties for contributors.