Resumen
Impulsivity-related measures have been obtained using operant conditioning tasks. Although it has been suggested that impulsivity indices can also be obtained using the elevated plus-maze (EPM), an anxiety-related animal test, no studies have examined the relationship between anxiety-related variables in EPM and impulsivity-related indices obtained during operant conditioning. Correlations between EPM measures and performance in a 60-s fixed-interval schedule of reinforcement (FI 60s) were established in this study. Twenty-four female rats were exposed to the EPM before starting training in the FI 60-s schedule. A positive correlation was found between the percentage of time spent in the central area of the EPM and both the FI efficiency index and the inter-response times. In addition, these three measures were positively correlated within Factor 1 of a factor analysis. No correlations were observed between open-arms measures in EPM and operant performance. These results suggest that time in the central area of the EPM may be a useful index of impulsivity in rodents without a pre-determined trait of impulsivity.
Bari, A., & Robbins, T. W. (2013). Inhibition and impulsivity: behavioral and neural basis of response control. Progress in Neurobiology, 108, 44-79. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.06.005
Berger, D. F., & Sagvolden, T. (1998). Sex differences in operant discrimination behaviour in an animal model of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Behavioural Brain Research, 94(1), 73-82.
Binder, E., Droste, S. K., Ohl, F., & Reul, J. M. H. M. (2004). Regular voluntary exercise reduces anxiety-related behaviour and impulsiveness in mice. Behavioural Brain Research, 155(2), 197-206. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2004.04.017
Carobrez, A. P., & Bertoglio, L. J. (2005). Ethological and temporal analyses of anxiety-like behavior: the elevated plus-maze model 20 years on. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 29(8), 1193-1205. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.04.017
Colorado, R. A., Shumake, J., Conejo, N. M., Gonzalez-Pardo, H., & Gonzalez-Lima, F. (2006). Effects of maternal separation, early handling, and standard facility rearing on orienting and impulsive behavior of adolescent rats. Behavioural Processes, 71(1), 51-58. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2005.09.007
Cruz, A. P., Frei, F., & Graeff, F. G. (1994). Ethopharmacological analysis of rat behavior on the elevated plus-maze. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 49(1), 171-176.
Dellu-Hagedorn, F. (2006). Relationship between impulsivity, hyperactivity and working memory: a differential analysis in the rat. Behavioral and Brain Functions: BBF, 2, 10. http://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-2-10
Ferguson, S. A., & Gray, E. P. (2005). Aging effects on elevated plus maze behavior in spontaneously hypertensive, Wistar-Kyoto and Sprague-Dawley male and female rats. Physiology & Behavior, 85(5), 621-628. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.06.009
Fernandes, C., & File, S. E. (1996). The influence of open arm ledges and maze experience in the elevated plus-maze. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 54(1), 31-40.
Gross, C., Zhuang, X., Stark, K., Ramboz, S., Oosting, R., Kirby, L., … Hen, R. (2002). Serotonin1A receptor acts during development to establish normal anxiety-like behaviour in the adult. Nature, 416(6879), 396-400. http://doi.org/10.1038/416396a
Hankosky, E. R., & Gulley, J. M. (2013). Performance on an impulse control task is altered in adult rats exposed to amphetamine during adolescence. Developmental Psychobiology, 55(7), 733-744. http://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21067
Kirkpatrick, K., Marshall, A. T., Clarke, J., & Cain, M. E. (2013). Environmental rearing effects on impulsivity and reward sensitivity. Behavioral Neuroscience, 127(5), 712-724. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0034124
Kishikawa, Y., Kawahara, Y., Yamada, M., Kaneko, F., Kawahara, H., & Nishi, A. (2014). The spontaneously hypertensive rat/Izm (SHR/Izm) shows attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder-like behaviors but without impulsive behavior: therapeutic implications of low-dose methylphenidate. Behavioural Brain Research, 274, 235-242. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2014.08.026
Langen, B., & Dost, R. (2011). Comparison of SHR, WKY and Wistar rats in different behavioural animal models: effect of dopamine D1 and alpha2 agonists. Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 3(1), 1-12. http://doi.org/10.1007/s12402-010-0034-y
Munn, R. G. K., & McNaughton, N. (2008). Effects of fluoxetine on hippocampal rhythmic slow activity and behavioural inhibition. Behavioural Pharmacology, 19(3), 257-264. http://doi.org/10.1097/FBP.0b013e3282ff1300
Pawlak, C. R., Karrenbauer, B. D., Schneider, P., & Ho, Y.-J. (2012). The elevated plus-maze test: differential psychopharmacology of anxiety-related behavior. Emotion Review, 4(1), 98-115. http://doi.org/10.1177/1754073911421374
Perry, G. M. L., Sagvolden, T., & Faraone, S. V. (2010). Intraindividual variability (IIV) in an animal model of ADHD - the Spontaneously Hypertensive Rat. Behavioral and Brain Functions: BBF, 6, 56. http://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-6-56
Riaño-Hernández, D., Guillen, A., & Buela-Casal, G. (2015). Conceptualización y evaluación de la impulsividad en adolescentes: una revisión sistemática. Universitas Psychologica, 14(3), 1077-1090. http://dx.doi.org/10.11144/Javeriana.upsy14-3.ceia
Rico, J. L., Penagos-Gil, M., Castañeda, A. F., & Corredor, K. (2016). Gerbils exhibit stable open-arms exploration across repeated testing on the elevated plus-maze. Behavioural Processes, 122, 104-109. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2015.11.017
Rodgers, R. J., & Johnson, N. J. (1995). Factor analysis of spatiotemporal and ethological measures in the murine elevated plus-maze test of anxiety. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 52(2), 297-303.
Sagvolden, T. (2000). Behavioral validation of the spontaneously hypertensive rat (SHR) as an animal model of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (AD/HD). Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24(1), 31-39.
Sorregotti, T., Mendes-Gomes, J., Rico, J. L., Rodgers, R. J., & Nunes-de-Souza, R. L. (2013). Ethopharmacological analysis of the open elevated plus-maze in mice. Behavioural Brain Research, 246, 76-85. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2013.02.035
Ueno, K. –., Togashi, H., Mori, K., Matsumoto, M., Ohashi, S., Hoshino, A., … Yoshioka, M. (2002). Behavioural and pharmacological relevance of stroke-prone spontaneously hypertensive rats as an animal model of a developmental disorder. Behavioural Pharmacology, 13(1), 1-13.
Wall, P. M., & Messier, C. (2001). Methodological and conceptual issues in the use of the elevated plus-maze as a psychological measurement instrument of animal anxiety-like behavior. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 25(3), 275-286.
Winstanley, C. A. (2011). The utility of rat models of impulsivity in developing pharmacotherapies for impulse control disorders. British Journal of Pharmacology, 164(4), 1301-1321. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1476-5381.2011.01323.x .
Esta revista científica se encuentra registrada bajo la licencia Creative Commons Reconocimiento 4.0 Internacional. Por lo tanto, esta obra se puede reproducir, distribuir y comunicar públicamente en formato digital, siempre que se reconozca el nombre de los autores y a la Pontificia Universidad Javeriana. Se permite citar, adaptar, transformar, autoarchivar, republicar y crear a partir del material, para cualquier finalidad (incluso comercial), siempre que se reconozca adecuadamente la autoría, se proporcione un enlace a la obra original y se indique si se han realizado cambios. La Pontificia Universidad Javeriana no retiene los derechos sobre las obras publicadas y los contenidos son responsabilidad exclusiva de los autores, quienes conservan sus derechos morales, intelectuales, de privacidad y publicidad. El aval sobre la intervención de la obra (revisión, corrección de estilo, traducción, diagramación) y su posterior divulgación se otorga mediante una licencia de uso y no a través de una cesión de derechos, lo que representa que la revista y la Pontificia Universidad Javeriana se eximen de cualquier responsabilidad que se pueda derivar de una mala práctica ética por parte de los autores. En consecuencia de la protección brindada por la licencia de uso, la revista no se encuentra en la obligación de publicar retractaciones o modificar la información ya publicada, a no ser que la errata surja del proceso de gestión editorial. La publicación de contenidos en esta revista no representa regalías para los contribuyentes.