El tiempo en el área central del laberinto en cruz elevado correlaciona con medidas relacionadas a impulsividad durante una tarea operante
PDF
HTML

Keywords

Anxiety
elevated plus-maze
impulsivity
operant conditioning
rats.

How to Cite

El tiempo en el área central del laberinto en cruz elevado correlaciona con medidas relacionadas a impulsividad durante una tarea operante. (2017). Universitas Psychologica, 15(5). https://doi.org/10.11144/Javeriana.upsy15-5.tcae
Almetrics
 
Dimensions
 

Google Scholar
 
Search GoogleScholar

Abstract

Medidas relacionadas a impulsividad se han obtenido mediante tareas de condicionamiento operante. Aunque se ha sugerido que índices de impulsividad pueden ser también obtenidos usando el laberinto en cruz elevado (LCE), una prueba animal relacionada con ansiedad, ningún estudio ha examinado la relación entre las variables de ansiedad en el LCE e índices de impulsividad obtenidos durante condicionamiento operante. En este estudio se establecieron correlaciones entre medidas del LCE y el desempeño en un programa de reforzamiento de intervalo fijo 60-s (IF 60-s). Diecisiete ratas hembras fueron expuestas la LCE antes de iniciar el entrenamiento en el programa de IF 60-s. Se encontró una correlación positiva entre el porcentaje de tiempo de permanencia en el área central del LCE y tanto el índice de eficiencia como los tiempos entre respuestas en IF. Adicionalmente, estas tres medidas correlacionaron de forma positiva dentro del Factor 1 del análisis factorial. No se observaron correlaciones entre las medidas de los brazos abiertos del LCE y la ejecución operante. Los resultados sugieren que el tiempo en el área central del LCE podría ser un índice útil de impulsividad en roedores sin un perfil predeterminado de impulsividad.

PDF
HTML

Bari, A., & Robbins, T. W. (2013). Inhibition and impulsivity: behavioral and neural basis of response control. Progress in Neurobiology, 108, 44-79. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.06.005

Berger, D. F., & Sagvolden, T. (1998). Sex differences in operant discrimination behaviour in an animal model of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Behavioural Brain Research, 94(1), 73-82.

Binder, E., Droste, S. K., Ohl, F., & Reul, J. M. H. M. (2004). Regular voluntary exercise reduces anxiety-related behaviour and impulsiveness in mice. Behavioural Brain Research, 155(2), 197-206. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2004.04.017

Carobrez, A. P., & Bertoglio, L. J. (2005). Ethological and temporal analyses of anxiety-like behavior: the elevated plus-maze model 20 years on. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 29(8), 1193-1205. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.04.017

Colorado, R. A., Shumake, J., Conejo, N. M., Gonzalez-Pardo, H., & Gonzalez-Lima, F. (2006). Effects of maternal separation, early handling, and standard facility rearing on orienting and impulsive behavior of adolescent rats. Behavioural Processes, 71(1), 51-58. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2005.09.007

Cruz, A. P., Frei, F., & Graeff, F. G. (1994). Ethopharmacological analysis of rat behavior on the elevated plus-maze. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 49(1), 171-176.

Dellu-Hagedorn, F. (2006). Relationship between impulsivity, hyperactivity and working memory: a differential analysis in the rat. Behavioral and Brain Functions: BBF, 2, 10. http://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-2-10

Ferguson, S. A., & Gray, E. P. (2005). Aging effects on elevated plus maze behavior in spontaneously hypertensive, Wistar-Kyoto and Sprague-Dawley male and female rats. Physiology & Behavior, 85(5), 621-628. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.06.009

Fernandes, C., & File, S. E. (1996). The influence of open arm ledges and maze experience in the elevated plus-maze. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 54(1), 31-40.

Gross, C., Zhuang, X., Stark, K., Ramboz, S., Oosting, R., Kirby, L., … Hen, R. (2002). Serotonin1A receptor acts during development to establish normal anxiety-like behaviour in the adult. Nature, 416(6879), 396-400. http://doi.org/10.1038/416396a

Hankosky, E. R., & Gulley, J. M. (2013). Performance on an impulse control task is altered in adult rats exposed to amphetamine during adolescence. Developmental Psychobiology, 55(7), 733-744. http://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21067

Kirkpatrick, K., Marshall, A. T., Clarke, J., & Cain, M. E. (2013). Environmental rearing effects on impulsivity and reward sensitivity. Behavioral Neuroscience, 127(5), 712-724. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0034124

Kishikawa, Y., Kawahara, Y., Yamada, M., Kaneko, F., Kawahara, H., & Nishi, A. (2014). The spontaneously hypertensive rat/Izm (SHR/Izm) shows attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder-like behaviors but without impulsive behavior: therapeutic implications of low-dose methylphenidate. Behavioural Brain Research, 274, 235-242. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2014.08.026

Langen, B., & Dost, R. (2011). Comparison of SHR, WKY and Wistar rats in different behavioural animal models: effect of dopamine D1 and alpha2 agonists. Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 3(1), 1-12. http://doi.org/10.1007/s12402-010-0034-y

Munn, R. G. K., & McNaughton, N. (2008). Effects of fluoxetine on hippocampal rhythmic slow activity and behavioural inhibition. Behavioural Pharmacology, 19(3), 257-264. http://doi.org/10.1097/FBP.0b013e3282ff1300

Pawlak, C. R., Karrenbauer, B. D., Schneider, P., & Ho, Y.-J. (2012). The elevated plus-maze test: differential psychopharmacology of anxiety-related behavior. Emotion Review, 4(1), 98-115. http://doi.org/10.1177/1754073911421374

Perry, G. M. L., Sagvolden, T., & Faraone, S. V. (2010). Intraindividual variability (IIV) in an animal model of ADHD - the Spontaneously Hypertensive Rat. Behavioral and Brain Functions: BBF, 6, 56. http://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-6-56

Riaño-Hernández, D., Guillen, A., & Buela-Casal, G. (2015). Conceptualización y evaluación de la impulsividad en adolescentes: una revisión sistemática. Universitas Psychologica, 14(3), 1077-1090. http://dx.doi.org/10.11144/Javeriana.upsy14-3.ceia

Rico, J. L., Penagos-Gil, M., Castañeda, A. F., & Corredor, K. (2016). Gerbils exhibit stable open-arms exploration across repeated testing on the elevated plus-maze. Behavioural Processes, 122, 104-109. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2015.11.017

Rodgers, R. J., & Johnson, N. J. (1995). Factor analysis of spatiotemporal and ethological measures in the murine elevated plus-maze test of anxiety. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 52(2), 297-303.

Sagvolden, T. (2000). Behavioral validation of the spontaneously hypertensive rat (SHR) as an animal model of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (AD/HD). Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24(1), 31-39.

Sorregotti, T., Mendes-Gomes, J., Rico, J. L., Rodgers, R. J., & Nunes-de-Souza, R. L. (2013). Ethopharmacological analysis of the open elevated plus-maze in mice. Behavioural Brain Research, 246, 76-85. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2013.02.035

Ueno, K. –., Togashi, H., Mori, K., Matsumoto, M., Ohashi, S., Hoshino, A., … Yoshioka, M. (2002). Behavioural and pharmacological relevance of stroke-prone spontaneously hypertensive rats as an animal model of a developmental disorder. Behavioural Pharmacology, 13(1), 1-13.

Wall, P. M., & Messier, C. (2001). Methodological and conceptual issues in the use of the elevated plus-maze as a psychological measurement instrument of animal anxiety-like behavior. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 25(3), 275-286.

Winstanley, C. A. (2011). The utility of rat models of impulsivity in developing pharmacotherapies for impulse control disorders. British Journal of Pharmacology, 164(4), 1301-1321. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1476-5381.2011.01323.x .

This journal is registered under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International Public License. Thus, this work may be reproduced, distributed, and publicly shared in digital format, as long as the names of the authors and Pontificia Universidad Javeriana are acknowledged. Others are allowed to quote, adapt, transform, auto-archive, republish, and create based on this material, for any purpose (even commercial ones), provided the authorship is duly acknowledged, a link to the original work is provided, and it is specified if changes have been made. Pontificia Universidad Javeriana does not hold the rights of published works and the authors are solely responsible for the contents of their works; they keep the moral, intellectual, privacy, and publicity rights. Approving the intervention of the work (review, copy-editing, translation, layout) and the following outreach, are granted through an use license and not through an assignment of rights. This means the journal and Pontificia Universidad Javeriana cannot be held responsible for any ethical malpractice by the authors. As a consequence of the protection granted by the use license, the journal is not required to publish recantations or modify information already published, unless the errata stems from the editorial management process. Publishing contents in this journal does not generate royalties for contributors.