Publicado may 18, 2017



PLUMX
Almetrics
 
Dimensions
 

Google Scholar
 
Search GoogleScholar


Diego Armando León Rodríguez

Fernando Cárdenas

##plugins.themes.bootstrap3.article.details##

Resumen

En las últimas décadas ha crecido el estudio los mecanismos involucrados en el comportamiento social, gran parte de estas indagaciones se han realizado desde una aproximación de la neurociencia social cognitiva, la cual se basa en un modelo representacional del procesamiento de información. No obstante, esta aproximación ha sido ampliamente criticada por desconocer la participación del cuerpo, la dinámica afectiva, el contexto social, el cambio durante el desarrollo y suponer un procesamiento modular endógeno. En este sentido, este artículo presenta un modelo neurodinámico de la cognición social (CS), comprendiéndola desde una aproximación enactiva, situada, relacional y sistémica. Desde este modelo se describen los principales cambios en esperados la actividad cerebral durante las interacciones sociales en tiempo real y durante la ontogenia. Se concluye resaltando los desafíos y oportunidades que este tipo de aproximaciones puede proporcionar a la neurociencia y psicología social del futuro.

Keywords

Enactive cognition, neurodynamic, social cognition, social development, social neuroscienceCognición social, cognición enactiva, desarrollo social, neurociencia social, neurodinámica.

References
Adolphs R. (1999). "Social Cognition and the Human Brain." Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 469-479.

Adolphs, R. (2001). The neurobiology of social cognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 11, 231–239.

Adolphs, R., Gosselin, F., Buchanan, T. W., Tranel, D., Schyns, P., & Damasio, A. R. (2005). A mechanism for impaired fear recognition after amygdala damage. Nature, 433, 68–72.

Adolphs, R. (2002). Neural systems for recognizing emotion. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 12(2), 169-177

Adolphs, R. (2008). Fear, faces, and the human amygdala. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18(2):166-72

Angelini M, Calbi M, Ferrari A, Sbriscia-Fioretti B, Franca M, Gallese V, et al. (2016) Proactive Control Strategies for Overt and Covert Go/NoGo Tasks: An Electrical Neuroimaging Study. PLoS ONE 11(3): e0152188. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0152188

Auyeung B., Lombardo M., Heinrichs M., Chakrabarti B., Sule, Deakin J., Bethlehem R., Dickens L., Mooney N., Sipple J., Thiemann P., & Baron-Cohen S. (2015). Oxytocin increases eye contact during a real-time, naturalistic social interaction in males with and without autism. Translational Psychiatry, 5, e507; doi:10.1038/tp.2014.146

Barrett L., & Satpute A., (2013). Large-scale brain networks in affective and social neuroscience: towards an integrative functional architecture of the brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23, 361–372.

Bachevalier J, & Meunier M., (2005). The neurobiology of social-emotional cognition in nonhuman primates. In: Easton A, Emery NJ, editors. The Cognitive Neuroscience of Social Behaviour. Psychology Press; London: pp. 19–57.

Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic children have a theory of mind?. Cognition, 21, 37-46.

Beer, J. S., & Ochsner, K. N. (2006). Social cognition: a multi level analysis. Brain Research 1079, 98–105. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.01.002

Bechara, A. (2004). The role of emotion in decision-making: evidence from neurological patients with orbitofrontal damage. Brain and Cognition, 55(1), 30–40.

Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Anderson, S. W. (1998). Dissociation of working memory from decision making within the human prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 18(1), 428–37.

Bischof, K., (1991). The Development of Empathy in Infants. In Lamb M. E. & Keller H. Infant Development: Perspectives from German Speaking Countries. Chap. 12, pp 245-273. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Berchicci, M., Zhang, T., Romero, L., Peters, A., Annett, R., et al. (2011). Development of mu rhythm in infants and preschool children. Developmental Neuroscience, 33, 130–143. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000329095

Blakemore, S-J. & Robbins, T.W. (2012). Decision-making in the adolescent brain. Nature Neuroscience 15, 1184-1191

Bonnet L., Comte A., Tatu L., Millot J.L., Moulin T., & Medeiros de Bustos E., (2015). The role of the amygdala in the perception of positive emotions: an "intensity detector". Frontiers Behavioral in Neuroscience. 7, 9:178. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00178.

Brosnan, S. F. & de Waal, F. B. M. (2003). Monkeys reject unequal pay. Nature 425, 297–-299

Cacioppo, J. T., & Berntson, G. G. (2002). Social neuroscience. En Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Adolphs, R., Carter, C. S., Davidson, R. J., McClintock, M. K., McEwen, B. S., Meaney, M. J., Schacter, D. L., Sternberg, E. M., Suomi, S. S. & Taylor, S. E. (Eds.). Foundations in social neuroscience (pp. 3-10). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Calder, A. J., & Young, A. W. (2005). Understanding the recognition of facial identity and facial expression. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 641–651.

Call, J., Braüer, J., Kaminski, J. & Tomasello, M. (2003). Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) are sensitive to the attentional state of humans. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117, 257-263.

Call J, & Tomasello M (2008). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Science, 12:187–192

Carlin & Calder (2013). The neural basis of eye gaze processing. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23, 450–455.

Carruthers, P., & Smith, P. (1996). (Eds). Theories of theories of mind. Cambridge: Cambridege University Press.

Choi-Kain L., Gunderson J., (2008). Mentalization: Ontogeny, Assessment, and Application in the Treatment of Borderline Personality Disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 1127–1135.

Cohen, J. R., Berkman, E. T., & Lieberman, M. D. (2011). Ventrolateral PFC as a self-control muscle and how to use it without trying. In D. T. Stuss & R. T. Knight (Eds.), Principles of Frontal Lobe Functions (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Coll, M-P., Bird, G., Catmur, C., & Press, C. (2014) Crossmodal repetition effects in the mu rhythm indicate tactile mirroring during action observation. Cortex, 63, 121-131.

Cunningham, W. A., Johnson, M. K., Raye, C. L., Chris Gatenby, J., Gore, J. C., & Banaji, M. R. (2004). Separable neural components in the processing of black and white faces. Psychological Science, 15, 806–13

Dadds, M., Jambrak, J., Pasalich, D., Hawes, D., & Brennan, J. (2011). Impaired attention to the eyes of attachment figures and the developmental origins of psychopathy. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(3), 238-245.

De Jaegher H, Di Paolo E, & Gallagher S (2010). Can social interaction constitute social cognition?. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(10), 441-447. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2010.06.009.

De Jaegher H, & Di Paolo E (2007). Participatory Sense-Making: An enactive approach to social cognition. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 6(4), 485-507.

De Gelder B. (2006). Towards the neurobiology of emotional body language. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(3), 242–249.

Decety, J. (2007). A social cognitive neuroscience model of human empathy. In E. Harmon-Jones and P. Winkielman (Eds.), Social Neuroscience: Integrating Biological and Psychological Explanations of Social Behavior (pp. 246-270). New York: Guilford Publications.

Decety J., (2015). The neural pathways, development and functions of empathy. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 3, 1–6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2014.12.001

Decety J., & Cacioppo, S., (2012). The speed of morality: a high-density electrical neuroimaging study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 108: 3068–3072, doi:10.1152/jn.00473.2012.

Decety J., & Cowell J.M., (2014). The complex relation between morality and empathy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 337-339.

DeGrazia (2009). "Self-Awareness in Animals," en Robert Lurz (ed.), The Philosophy of Animal Minds. (pp. 201-217), Cambridge University Press.

Dennett, D. (1978). Cognition and consciousness in non human species. Commentary. Behavioral & Brain Science, 4, 568-569.

Devue, C., Collette, F., Balteau, E., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A., Maquet, P. & Brédart, S. (2007). ‘Here I am: The cortical correlates of visual selfrecognition’, Brain Research, 1143, 169-182.

Di Paolo, E. D., & De Jaegher, H. D. (2012). The interactive brain hypothesis. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6:163. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00163

Dumas, G., Nadel, J., Soussignan, R., Martinerie, J., & Garnero, L., (2010). Inter-brain synchronization during social interaction. PLoS ONE 5 (8), e12166.

Emery N., (2009). The evolution of social cognition. Chapter 5 in: The Cognitive Neuroscience of Social Behaviour, N. Emery and A. Easton (eds.) Philadelphia: Psychology Press.

Emery N., & Clayton N., (2009). Comparative Social Cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 60:87–113. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163526.

Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2004). Why rejection hurts: A common neural alarm system for physical pain and social pain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 294 – 300.

Eisenberger, N. I., Taylor, S. E., Gable, S. L., Hilmert, C. J., & Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Neural pathways link social support to attenuated neuroendocrine stress response. NeuroImage, 35 , 1601 – 1612.

Eisenberger N. (2013). Social ties and health: a social neuroscience perspective. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23,407–413.

Ekman, P. (1994). All Emotions Are Basic. En P. Ekman & R. Davidson, The Nature of Emotion: Fundamental Questions. Pp. 56-58. New York: Oxford University Press.

Easton, A. & Emery, N. J. (Eds.) (2005). The cognitive neuroscience of social behavior. Hove, East Sussex, Reino Unido: Psychology Press.

Engen H., & Singer T., (2013). Empathy circuits. Haakon G. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23,275–282

Fadiga, L., Craighero, L., Buccino, G., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Short communication: Speech listening specifically modulates the excitability of tongue muscles: A TMS study. European Journal of Neuroscience,15, 399–402.

Fleischer, S., & Slotnick BM (1978). Disruption of maternal behavior in rats with lesions of the septal area. Physiology Behaviour. 21, 189–200.

Fonagy, P., Gergeley G, Jurist E, & Target M., (2002). Affect Regulation, Mentalization, and the Development of the Self. New York, Other Press.

Frith, C., (2008) Social cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 363, 2033-2039 doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0005.

Frith C., & Frith U., (2012). Mechanisms of Social Cognition. Annual Review in Psychology. 63, 287–313.

Fusar-Poli, P., Placentino, A., Carletti, F., Landi, P., Allen, P., Surguladze, S., … & Politi, P., (2009). Functional atlas of emotional faces processing: a voxel-based meta-analysis of 105 functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Journal of Psychiatry Neuroscieces. 34, 418–432.

Gallagher, H. L., Jack, A. I., Roepstorff, A., & Frith, C. D. (2002). Imaging the intentional stance in a competitive game. Neuroimage 16,814–21

Gallup, G.G., Jr. (1970). Chimpanzees: Self Recognition. Science, 167, 86-87.

Gopnik, A., & Wellman, H. (1992). Why the child´s theory of mind really is a theory. Mind and Language, 7(1-2), 145-171.

Goldman A., & Vignemont F., (2009) Is social cognition embodied? Trends in Cognitive Science, 13 (4). doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.007.

Grande-García (2009). Neurociencia social: El maridaje entre la psicología social y las neurociencias cognitivas. Revisión e introducción a una nueva disciplina. Anales de psicología, 25(1) 1-20.

Greene, J., & Haidt, J. (2002). How (and where) does moral judgment work?. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 517-523.

Greene, J. D., Nystrom, L. E. , Engell, A. D., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2006). The neural basis of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgement. Neuron, 44, 389-400.

Greene J. (2005). Emotion and cognition in moral judgment: evidence from neuroimaging. In Changeux JP, Damasio AR, Singer R, Christen Y, eds. Neurobiology of human values. Berlin: Springer-Verlag; p. 57-67.

Grossmann, T., & Johnson, M. (2007). The development of the social brain in infancy. European Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 909-919.

Grossman, E., Donnelly, M., Price, R., Pickens, D., Morgan, V., Neighbor, G., & Blake, R. (2000). Brain areas involved in perception of biological motion. Journal in Cognitive Neurosciences. 12, 711–720

Haggard, P. (2008) Human volition: towards a neuroscience of will. Nature reviews: neuroscience. 9 (12), pp. 934-946 Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.

Harmon-Jones, E. & Winkielman, P. (2007). Social Neuroscience. Integrating biological and psychological explanations of social behavior. Guilford Press. New York.

Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2001). Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know? Animal Behaviour, 61, 139–151.

Hatfield, E. & Rapson, R. L. (2000). Emotional contagion. In Craighead W. E., & Nemeroff C. B (Eds.). The Corsini encyclopedia of psychology and behavioral science. New York: John Wiley & Sons, 493-495.

Haxby J. V., & Gobbini M. I. (2011). Distributed Neural Systems for Face Perception, in The Oxford Handbook of Face Perception, Eds Calder A. J., Rhodes G., Johnson M., editors. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 93–110.

Heatherton, T. F. 2011. Neuroscience of self and self-regulation. Annual Review in Psychology. 62: 363-90.

Heberlein, A. S., & Adolphs, R. (2005). Functional anatomy of human social cognition. In: The Cognitive Neuroscience of Social Behaviour, N. Emery and A. Easton (eds.) Philadelphia: Psychology Press.

Heberlein AS, & Adolphs R. (2007). Neurobiology of emotion recognition: Current evidence for shared substrates. In: Social Neuroscience: Integrating Biological and Psychological Explanations of Social Behavior, E. Harmon-Jones and P. Winkielman (eds.) New York: Guilford Press.

Hein, G., & Knight, R.T., (2008). Superior temporal sulcus — it's my area: or is it? Journal in Cognitive Neurosciences. 20 (12), 2125–2136

Herrmann, E., Keupp, S., Hare, B., Vaish, A., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Direct and indirect reputation formation in non-human great apes and human children. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 127(1), 63-75.

Hesse E., Mikulan E., Decety J., Sigman M., Garcia MC., Silva W., … & Ibanez A. (2016). Early detection of intentional harm in the human amygdala. Brain, 139(1), 54-61. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv336

Hu J., Cao Y., Blue F., & Zhou X., (2014). Low social status decreases the neural salience of unfairness. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 402.

Hooker, C., & Park, S. (2005). You must be looking at me: The nature of gaze perception in schizophrenia patients. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 10, 327–345.

Iacoboni M., (2009). Las neuronas espejo: Empatía, neuropolítica, autismo, imitación, o de cómo entendemos a los otros. Katz editores.

Iacoboni, M. & Dapretto, (2006). The mirror neuron system and the consequences of its dysfunction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 7, 942-951.

Ibañez A., Melloni M., & Huepe D., (2012). What event-related potentials (ERPs) bring to social neuroscience?. Social Neuroscience, 7, 632-49.

Izard, C. E., Woodburn, E. M., & Finlon, K. J. (2010). Extending emotion science to the study of discrete emotions in infants. Emotion Review, 2, 134–136. doi: 10.1177/1754073909355003

Jabbi, M., Swart, M., & Keysers, C. (2007). Empathy for positive and negative emotions in the gustatory cortex. NeuroImage, 34, 1744– 1753

Jack A., Dawson A., & Norr M., (2013). Seeing human: Distinct and overlapping neural signatures associated with two forms of dehumanization. NeuroImage 79, 313–328.

Kerr, M., van Zalk, M., & Stattin, H. (2012). Psychopathic traits moderate peer influence on adolescent delinquency. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 826-835.

Kessler, K., & Thomson, L. A. (2010). The embodied nature of spatial perspective taking: embodied transformation versus sensorimotor interference. Cognition, 114(1), 72–88.

Keysers, C. Wicker B., Gazzola V., Anton J., Fogassi L., & Gallese V., (2004). A Touching Sight: SII/PV Activation during the Observation and Experience of Touch. Neuron, 42, 335–346.

Kirk, U., Downar, J., &Montague, P. R. (2011). Interoception drives increased rational decision-making in meditators playing the Ultimatum Game. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 5:49

Koike T., Tanabeb H., & Sadato N., (2015). Hyperscanning neuroimaging technique to reveal the “two-in-one” system in social interactions. Neuroscience Research, 90, 25–32.

Johnson, M. H., (2005). Subcortical face processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 766–774.

Johnson, M. H., & Farroni, T. (2003). Perceiving and acting on the eyes: The development and neural basis of eye gaze perception. In O. Pascalis & A. Slater (Eds.), The development of face processing in infancy and early childhood: Current perspectives (pp. 155–168). New York: Nova Science Publishers.

Johansson, G. (1973) Percept. Psychophys. 14, 201–211.

Kuhlmeier V. A., Troje N. F., & Lee V., (2010). “Young infants detect the direction of biological motion in point-light displays,”. Infancy, 15(1), 83–93.

Kumsta R. & Heinrichs M., (2013). Oxytocin, stress and social behavior: neurogenetics of the human oxytocin system. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23:11–16

León D. (2010). El Carácter Afectivo de la Consciencia. En Roso J. & Pérez A. El Reto de La Conciencia. Respuestas desde la psicología y la neurociencia. Editorial PSICOM Editores, Bogotá, Colombia.

León Diego A. (2012) Afectividad y Conciencia: la experiencia subjetiva de los valores biológicos. Revista Chilena de neuropsicología. 7(3) 108-114.

León Diego A (2013). Introducción a la cognición social y la neurociencia social cognitiva. Contextos en Psicología, 9, 1-12.

León D., & Martinez F., (2009). ¿Pueden las Neuronas Espejo Ser una Respuesta al Nacimiento de la Autoconsciencia?. Poliantea. 9, 55-69.

Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1988). Autistic children’s understanding of seeing, knowing and believing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 315-324.

Liao Y., Acar Z., Makeig S, & Deak G., (2015). EEG imaging of toddlers during dyadic turn-taking: Mu-rhythm modulation while producing or observing social actions. NeuroImage, 112, 52–60.

Libet, B. (1983). Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readinesspotential). The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. Brain 106, 623–642.

Lieberman, M. D. (2007a). Social cognitive neuroscience. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Social Psychology. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Press.

Lieberman, M. D. (2007b). Social cognitive neuroscience: A review of core processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 259-89.

Lieberman, M. D. (2010). Social cognitive neuroscience. S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & G. Lindzey (Eds). Handbook of Social Psychology (5th ed.) (pp. 143-193). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Lieberman M. D. (2012a). Education and the social brain. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 1, 3–9.

Lieberman, M. D. (2012b). Self-knowledge: From philosophy to neuroscience to psychology. S. Vazire & T. D. Wilson (Eds.) Handbook of Self-knowledge (pp. 63-76). New York: Guilford.

Lieberman, M. D. (2013). Peaks and Valleys. In Lieberman Social: why our brains are wired to connect. Ed. Crown Publishers. New York.

Liu, T., & Pelowski, M., (2014). Clarifying the interaction types in two-person neuro-science research. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 276.

Luo, Y., & Baillargeon, R. (2007). Do 12.5-month-old infants consider what objects others can see when interpreting their actions? Cognition, 105(3), 489–512.

Mascaro J., Rilling J., Negi L., & Raison C., (2013). Compassion meditation enhances empathic accuracy and related neural activity. SCAN 8, 48-55.

Maister L., Banissy M., & Tsakiris M., (2013). Mirror-touch synaesthesia changes representations of self-identity. Neuropsychologia, 51, 802–808.

Marsh, A., Crowe, S., Yu, H., Gorodetsky, E., Goldman, D., & Blair, R. (2011a). Serotonin Transporter Genotype (5-HTTLPR) Predicts Utilitarian Moral Judgments, PLoS ONE, 6 (10) DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0025148

Marsh, A. A., Finger, E. C., Fowler, K. A., Jurkowitz, I. T., Schechter, J. C., Yu, H. H., Pine, D. S., & Blair, R. J. R. (2011b). Reduced amygdala-orbitofrontal connectivity during moral judgments in youths with disruptive behavior disorders and psychopathic traits. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 194, 279-286.

Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development, 77, 622–646.

Morelli, S.A., Rameson, L.T., & Lieberman, M.D. (2014). The neural components of empathy: predicting daily prosocial behavior. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 9, 39–47.

Moll J., Zahn R., de Oliveira-Souza R., Bramati I.E., Krueger F., Tura B., Cavanagh A.L., & Grafman J., (2011). Impairment of prosocial sentiments is associated with frontopolar and septal damage in frontotemporal dementia. Neuroimage. 54(2), 1735-42. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.026. Epub 2010 Aug 20.

Moll, J., De Oliveira-Souza, R., & Zahn, R. (2008). The neural basis of moral cognition: sentiments, concepts, and values. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124(N/A), 161-180. [N/A]. 10.1196/annals.1440.005

Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Level I perspective-taking at 24 months of age. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 603–613.

Möhring N, Brandt E., Mohr B., Pulvermüller F., & Neuhaus A., (2014). ERP adaptation provides direct evidence for early mirror neuron activation in the inferior parietal lobule. International Journal of Psychophysiology,. 94(1), 76-83. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.07.001.

Mukamel, R., Ekstrom, A. D., Kaplan, J., Iacoboni, M. & Fried, I. (2010). Single neuron responses in humans during execution and observation of actions. Current Biology 20(8), 750–56.

Narvaez D., (2012). Moral Neuroeducation from Early Life Through the Lifespan. Neuroethics 5, 145–157. DOI 10.1007/s12152-011-9117-5

Nyström, P., Ljunghammar, T., Rosander, K., & von Hofsten, C., (2011). Using mu rhythm desynchronization to measure mirror neuron activity in infants. Developmental Science, 14, 327–335.

Nummenmaa, L., Passamonti, L., Rowe, J. B., Engell, A. D., & Calder, A. J. (2009). Connectivity analysis reveals a cortical network for eye gaze perception. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 1780-1787.

Oberman & Ramachandran (2007). The Simulating Social Mind: The Role of the Mirror Neuron System and Simulation in the Social and Communicative Deficits of Autism Spectrum Disorders. Psychological Bulletin, 133(2), 310–327.

Olivares E. & Iglesias J., (2000). Bases neurales de la percepción y el reconocimiento de caras. Revista de Neurología, 30(10), 946-952.

Olivares E., Saavedra C., & Iglesias J., (2012). Potenciales evocados como marcadores neurofisiológicos de la percepción y el reconocimiento de caras. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 44 (2), 27-38.

Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective Neuroscience. New York: Oxford University Press.

Peake S., Dishion T., Stormshak E., Moore W., & Pfeifer J., (2013). Risk-taking and social exclusion in adolescence: Neural mechanisms underlying peer influences on decision-making. NeuroImage 82 (2013) 23–34.

Pelphrey K, Adolphs R, & Morris JP (2004). Neuroanatomical substrates of social cognition dysfunction in autism. Mental retardation and developmental disabilities research reviews, 10(4), 259-71.

Platek, S. M. & Thomson, J. W. (2007). Neural correlates of facial resemblance. En S. M. Platek, J. P. Keenan y T. K. Shackelford (Eds.), Evolutionary cognitive neuroscience (pp. 221-243). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Plotnik, J. M., de Waal, F. B. M., & Reiss, D. (2006). Self-recognition in an Asian elephant. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 103.

Povinelli, D. J., Landau, K. R. & Perilloux, H. K. (1996). Self-recognition in young children using delayed versus live feedback: evidence of a developmental asynchrony. Child Development, 67, 1540-1554.

Premack D, & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does chimpanzee have a theory of mind?. Behavioral Brain Science. 4, 9-30.

Premack, D. (1988). 'Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind' revisited. In R. W. Byrne (Ed.), Machiavellian intelligence: Social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans (pp. xiv, 413). New York, NY, US: Clarendon Press/Oxford University Press.

Ramachandran V., (2011). The Tell-Tale Brain: Unlocking the Mystery of Human Nature. William Heinemann.

Reiss, D & Marino, L (2001). Mirror self-recognition in the bottlenose dolphin: A case of cognitive convergence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 98, 5937-5942.

Richardson, M. J., Dale, R., & Marsh, K. (2014). “Complex dynamical systems in social and personality psychology: theory, modeling and analysis,” in Handbook of Research Methods in Social and Personality Psychology, eds H. T. Reis and C. M. Judd (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 253–282

Righi, G., & Nelson, C.A. (2012). The neural architecture and developmental course of face processing. In P. Rakic & J. Rubenstein (Eds.), Comprehensive developmental neuroscience. San Diego, CA: Elsevier.

Rilling, J. K., Sanfey, A. G., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). The Neural Correlates of Theory of Mind within Interpersonal Interactions. Neuroimage, 22(4), 1694-1703.

Rizzolatti, G., & Arbib, M. A. (1998). Language within our grasp. Trends in Neurosciences, 21, 188–194.

Rizzolatti G, & Craighero L., (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.

Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review, 110, 145172.

Rhodes G., Pond S., Burton N., Kloth N., Jeffery L., Bell J., … & Palermo R., (2015). How distinct is the coding of face identity and expression. Cognition, 142, 123-127.

Rushworth M., Mars R., & Sallet J., (2013). Are there specialized circuits for social cognition and are they unique to humans?. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23:436–442.

Ruttan R., McDonnell M., & Nordgren L., (2015). Having “Been There” Doesn’t Mean I Care: When Prior Experience Reduces Compassion for Emotional Distress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108(4), 610–622.

Sanfey A.G., Rilling J., Aronson J., Nystrom L., & Cohen J. (2003) “The Neural Basis of Economic Decision Making,”. Science, 300, 1755-1758.

Schilbach L, Timmermans B, Reddy V, Costall A, Bente G, Schlicht T, & Vogeley K. (2013). Toward a second-person neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(4):393-414. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12000660.

Segura A. & Gutiérrez G., (2006). Cooperación en Ratas: Efectos de la Experiencia Temprana. Revista Interamericana de Psicología/Interamerican Journal of Psychology, 40(2), 241-252.

Semendeferi, K., Schleicher, A., Zilles, K., Armstrong, E., & Van Hoesen, G. W. (2001). Evolution of The hominoid prefrontal cortex: imaging and quantitative analysis of area 10. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 114, 224‐241.

Shirtcliff, E. A., M. J. Vitacco, A. R. Graf, A. J. Gostisha, J. L. Merz, & C. Zahn-Waxler. 2009. “Neurobiology of empathy and callousness: Implications for the development of antisocial behavior.” Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 27, 137-171.

Singer, T., Seymour, B., O ’ Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R., & Frith, C. D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science, 303, 1157 – 1162.

Singer, T., Seymour, B., O ’ Doherty, J. P., Stephan, K. E., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2006). Empathetic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature, 439, 466 – 469.

Schulze, L., Renneberg, B., & Lobmaier, J. S. (2013). Gaze perception in social anxiety and social anxiety disorder. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience.

Sodian, B., Thoermer, C., & Metz, U. (2007). Now I see it but you don’t: 14-montholds can represent another person’s visual perspective. Developmental Science, 10, 199–204.

Spunt, R., Meyer M., & Lieberman M., (2015). The Default Mode of Human Brain Function Primes the Intentional Stance. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 27, 6, 1116–1124

Susilo, T., & Duchaine, B., (2013). Advances in developmental prosopagnosia research. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23, 423–429.

Stellar J., Cohen A., Oveis C., Keltner D., (2015). Affective and Physiological Responses to the Suffering of Others: Compassion and Vagal Activity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108 (4), 572–585

Taylor, V., Daneault V., Grant J., Scavone G., Breton E., Roffe-Vidal S., … & Beauregard M., (2013). Impact of meditation training on the default mode network during a restful state. SCAN 8, 4-14.

Tabibnia G, Satpute AB, Lieberman MD. (2008). The sunny side of fairness. Psychological Science, 19:339.

Tabibnia, G. & Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Fairness and cooperation are rewarding: Evidence from social cognitive neuroscience. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1118, 90-101.

Tabibnia, G., Satpute A. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2008) The sunny side of fairness: preference for fairness activates reward circuitry (and disregarding unfairness activates self-control circuitry). Psychological Science, 19(4), 339–347

Tamietto, M., & de Gelder, B. (2010). Neural bases of the non-conscious perception of emotional signals. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 697-709.

Tirapu J., Pérez G., Erekatxo M., & Pelegrín C. (2007) “Qué es la teoría de la mente”?. Revista de Neurología. 44, 479-89.

Todorov A., Mende-Siedlecki P., & Dotsch R. (2013). Social judgments from faces. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 23, 373–380.

Tomasello M., & Vaish A., (2013). Origins of Human Cooperation and Morality. Annual Review in Psychology, 64, 231–55.

Tsakiris, M., Prabhu, G., & Haggard, P. (2006). Having a body versus moving your body: How agency structures body-ownership. Consciousness and Cognition, 15, 423–432.

Tylén, K., Allen, M., Hunter, B. K., & Roepstorff, A. (2012). Interaction vs. observation: distinctive modes of social cognition in human brain and behavior? A combined fMRI and eye-tracking study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 6, 331. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00331

Uddin, L. Q., Iacoboni, M., Lange, C., & Keenan, J. P. (2007). The self and social cognition: the role of cortical midline structures and mirror neurons. Trends in Cognitive Science, 11, 153–157. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.01.001

Umiltà, C., (2007). Consciousness and Control of Action. En Zelazo, Moscovitch & Thompson. The Cambridge Handbook of Consciousness. Cambridge University Press.

van Leijenhorst, L. & Crone, E. A. (2009). Paradoxes in adolescent risk-taking. In: Zelazo, P. D, Chandler, M. & Crone, E. A. (Eds). Developmental Social Cognitive Neuroscience. Psychology Press.

van Leijenhorst, L., Gunther Moor, B., Op de Macks, Z.A., Rombouts, S.A.R.B., Westenberg, P. M., & Crone, E.A. (2010). Adolescent risky decision making: Neurocognitive development of reward and control regions. NeuroImage, 51, 345-355.

Voos, A. & Pelphrey, K. (2013). Decety, J., & Michalska, K. J. Developmental neuroscience of social perception. In P. Rakic and J. Rubenstein (Eds.), Comprehensive Developmental Neuroscience. Oxford: Elsevier.

Walter NT, Montag C, Markett S, Felten A, Voigt G, & Reuter M. (2012). Ignorance is no excuse: moral judgments are influenced by a genetic variation on the oxytocin receptor gene. Brain Cognition, 78:268-273.

Wang J., Nicol T., Skoe E., Sams., & Kraus N., (2010). Emotion and the auditory brainstem response to speech. Neuroscience Letters 469, 319-323.

Wiese, H., Kache,l U., & Schweinberger, S., (2013). Holistic face processing of own- and other-age faces in young and older adults: ERP evidence from the composite face task. NeuroImage 74, 306–317.

Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining functions of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128.

White, S. F., Brislin, S., Sinclair, S., Fowler, K. A., Pope, K., & Blair. R. J. R. (2013). The relationship between large cavum septum pellucidum and antisocial behavior, callousunemotional traits, and psychopathy in adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 575-581.

Wu N, Li Z, & Su Y. (2012). The association between oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism (OXTR) and trait empathy. Affective Disorders 138 (3), 468-72. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.01.009.

Wu Y., Zang Y., Yuan B., & Tian X., (2015). Neural correlates of decision making after unfair treatment. Frontiersin Human Neuroscience. 9, 123.

Young L., & Dungan J., (2012). Where in the brain is morality? Everywhere and maybe nowhere. Social Neuroscience, 7:1, 1-10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2011.569146 .

Zaki, J., Hennigan, K., Weber, J., & Ochsner, K. N. (2010). Social cognitive conflict resolution: contributions of domain-general and domain-specific neural systems. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 8481–88

Zhen Z, Fang H, & Liu J., (2013). The Hierarchical Brain Network for Face Recognition. PLoS ONE, 8(3): e59886. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0059886

Zuberbuhler K., (2008). Gaze following. Current in Biology, 18, 453–455. Doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.03.015.
Cómo citar
León Rodríguez, D. A., & Cárdenas, F. (2017). Aproximación Neurodinámica a la Cognición Social. Universitas Psychologica, 15(5). https://doi.org/10.11144/Javeriana.upsy15-5.ancs
Sección
Artículos

Artículos más leídos del mismo autor/a